SEB AR RT Sr Sa AREA A we erie » verve - HS PRT WE 0 THC BAS PTI Hp a ; i SERRA po 3, Sel Be Ca a4 SE ir SER FER aT 5 Nik ; : : : ea . : : bo ‘ : : 4 a : : s 5 ; : QrACEARS Es AAA pin Sf ye Pn = pa PR CAT ri rhe ; os 7) y ) li Ka : ES alas } ESE LU) I) 3 i KE PY Loui 44 2 ARE A ws REO Fd hg LAGER a 4 A 0 is Sh : ’ + 5 . ESOT CI A OG X , A : id) LAL Sp AR RS CE GTR g (pS a :; Seda LL LEERY Sieh Pun a Hoist pre Wy THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER BY THE SAME AUTHOR THE CHILD'S ATTITUDE TO LIFE CHILDREN'S DREAMS THE CHILD IN THE CHANGING HOME THE MENTAL AND PHYSICAL WELFARE OF THE CHILD THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER BY C. W. KIMMINS M.A., D.Sc. METHUEN & CO. LTD. 36 ESSEX STREET W.C. LONDON Fiyst Published tn 1928 PRINTED IN GREAT BRITAIN PREFACE HE subject of laughter is an intensely interesting study. It has attracted the attention of dis- tinguished philosophers, psychologists, physiologists, l'ducational journalists and various types of social reformers. [he result is that the literature of laughter, in its extra- rdinary variety, represents an abnormally well-worked lield by expert explorers. To enter such a field needs, {herefore, something in the nature of an apology. My 'xcuse must be that, from a series of investigations of the ctivities of young children, I have been much impressed y the fact that in many theories of laughter insufficient mportance has been attached to the following points : I) the child’s contribution to the subject ; (2) the need of reater emphasis of the play-attitude ; (3) the changes in he appreciation of the type of humorous experiences at ifferent ages and (4) the social attitude towards elements thich find expression in laughter at different epochs of evelopment in the history of the race. The fact that the ‘sudden glory’ theory of Hobbes Jiould have maintained such a supremacy among the 1eories of laughter for so long a period affords ample vidence that insufficient thought had been given to the lrlier stages of child life in the framing of the theory. lany writers in discussing humour and laughter have voided coming to any conclusion as to the exact nature of 1e subject, and when a philosopher of the standing of \lobbes embodied his views in a clear-cut definition of ughter there was a natural tendency for those who lowed him to take shelter under this authoritative con- ete statement as a starting point. This theory thus v | 230 vi THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER attained a position which it never deserved, and it rota the development of more catholic views on laughter'e: many years. Further points in the consideration of laughter wie also require more emphasis are the physical and emotiu condition of the laugher and the nature of the environnps in which the humorous situation arises. The compara success of that modern terror, the professional humoris i largely due to his power of gauging adequately the ate sphere in which he finds himself and the nature of Bo wares in his stock of jokes and funny stories which are nt suitable for the particular occasion. With such a rich literature as that of laughter it weld not be possible to enumerate the various sources to wii any writer on the subject must necessarily be mh Some of the more important of these will be referred td different sections of this book. Of the more recent boki on humour and laughter I should, however, acknowless my special appreciation of one by Mr. Max Eastruz (published in 1921) on The Sense of Humour, and thast Mr. J. Y. T. Greig (published in 1923) on The Psychologss Laughter and Comedy, in which is given a very valuable, i 3 fairly exhaustive, historical summary of the various the of laughter. 4 In the numerous stories recorded by the children reacst will come across many old friends, but my object has bi simply to indicate by them the appeal made to the senst humour of the child in different stages of development Again, I must express my sincere thanks to very me! teachers in England and America, and to a small n in France and Germany, for the generous assistance tly have given to me in carrying out my studies of children I must also thank my friend, Mr. A. J. Mundella, for 1 3 help he has given to me in the tiresome task of prof correction. 8 C.W.Ki March 1928 § CONTENTS APTER PREFACE ; 3 : . ; . ; I. INTRODUCTION I. THEORIES OF LAUGHTER neh (Laughter in the seventeenth and eighteenth centuries) [I, THEORIES OF LAUGHTER (Laughter in the nineteenth century) ‘V. THEORIES OF LAUGHTER (Laughter in the twentieth century) . LAUGHTER AND DREAMS a LAUGHTER AND TEARS + THE SPIRIT OF THE CHILD THE BEGINNINGS OF LAUGHTER WHAT YOUNG CHILDREN LAUGH AT (Five to twelve years of age) THE LAUGHTER OF OLDER CHILDREN (Twelve to eighteen years of age) ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR [. THE LAUGHTER OF COLOURED CHILDREN PAGE 34 53 59 67 76 88 105, 126 169 TO MY WIFE ome SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION I is interesting to notice how, in the literature of laughter, the more ambitious contributors have vainly attempted to bring the varieties of forms of humorous experiences under some definite laughter-causing principle. Surprise, followed by a relief from restraint; derision, | accompanied by a feeling of superiority ; amusement at . the misfortunes of others ; the incongruous ; the economy of psychic expenditure; the machine-like behaviour of living things ; the joy of the release of energy for no useful . purpose, and so on, all have their votaries. In order to . bring causes of laughter into the desired category attempts | are made which are, in themselves, frequently humorous. | Much of the literature consists of the criticism of the work of other labourers in the field, by giving illustrations which . \ cannot be reconciled with the theory under review. It “1s a hopeless task, especially in children’s laughter, to 1 secure anything approaching a common principle. More- Lover, the humorous elements which provoke laughter most 1 readily vary from age to age. The types mentioned above are undoubtedly all available | for the purpose of mirth-production, but they are simply ) conditions which, in suitable circumstances, may give the i response of laughter. In many cases more than one factor ‘may be present in the same humorous situation. In fact, < as has been pointed out elsewhere ! the life of a good story 1 Kimmins, The Child's Attitude to Life. I I \ = aT rm 2 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER is prolonged if it contains more than one constituent of o humorous appeal. The difficulties of classification of the causes of laughter 2 disappear when its instinctive nature is fully grasped. Not a only is laughter an instinct, but its close association with the play-attitude is manifest to every intelligent observer 3 of children. It is unfortunate that much of the most'2 highly skilled observation of children has been concerned with the neurotic flotsam and jetsam of the consulting room, rather than with healthy normal material. The}: joyous natural laughter of happy children is rarely heard in the clinics for pathological treatment. This has not @ been without its effect upon the theories of laughter. The connexion of the smile with the laugh is conceded of by most authorities. The appearance of the laugh about & two months later is, in virtue of its more complex muscular & basis, what might well be expected. Professor Sully’s:} account of the development of the laugh from the smile, ass described in his Essay on Laughter is reasonable, ands nothing can be gained by attempting to divorce them. a The laugh of the child is characteristic and when fully gl developed undergoes comparatively little change. In as group of children in the nursery, if one laughs the nurse % outside has no difficulty in identifying the laugher though * no word is spoken. In adults the laugh is frequently more readily recognized than the voice. A Cambridge graduate » on revisiting Trinity after an interval of five years was surprised that his bed-maker, hearing him laugh with some 3 friends near to his old rooms, at once recognized him, & though she had not seen him and his visit was unexpected. ] His friends did not share his surprise. 1 The change in the social attitude from period to period f has undoubtedly materially affected the kind of humorous # experience which causes laughter, and this naturally in- % creases the difficulty of comparing, with any advantage, the 3 definitions of laughter at different epochs in the development # I AAR co vrai INTRODUCTION 3 of a cultured outlook on life. The type of material which causes amusement must necessarily vary with the stage of civilization, and more especially with the attitude of class to class in the structure of society. From the descriptions of laughter of the seventeenth century, it is clear that physical deformities were the subject of laughter to a far greater extent than is common in the present century. The sight of a crippled child has ceased to be an appropriate subject of merriment, and the laughter of the onlooker would be severely condemned. Practically the only subject of deformity which can still be considered a legitimate source of laughter is the abnormally fat man or woman—if obesity can be considered a deformity. In the description of humorous incidents by young children of minor accidents in the streets, the story is certain of a much more generous reception if the sufferer was a fat man. The humorous appeal which results in laughter must also depend upon the background of knowledge of the person or group affected. The story which never fails to secure . a satisfactory response in a cultured audience would probably be received in chilling silence in the bar-room of a country inn; whereas, one of the more primitive type with local colouring would be sure of uproarious approval. Similarly, in the revival of an early English farce or comedy, in which some of the humorous situations are of a simple character, no enthusiasm is aroused in the cultured adult, but the merry laughter of young children in the audience leaves no doubt as to its appeal to them. The springs of laughter can thus be stimulated in a variety of ways, and to a much wider extent than is consistent with grouping them into a class bearing the hall-mark of a particular variety of humorous situation in order to make them function. Freedom of thought has a powerful effect in extending the field of observation in appreciating the real nature of TE AE spa, iors _ welcomed by the physiologist as well as the psychologist. 4 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER phenomena. A sense of humour, in this connexion, is 0: extraordinary value, not only in the wider outlook it: gives on life, but also in the added happiness which results li in being able to view a trying situation from many angles, 3 A man with no sense of humour is limited in his view. Hel is, as it were, wearing blinkers, and has less capacity for? solving difficulties, for he can only look in one direction Psychiatrists have the highest opinion of the value of the sense of humour, and their difficulties experienced in‘ bringing about a solution of difficult mental problems are! frequently associated with the absence of a sense of humours in the unfortunate subjects under their treatment. | From the physiological point of view there can be Jol { doubt as to the yalue of laughter. It deepens the inspira- tions, quickens the circulation, and there is a large mass of evidence available to show that it improves the digestion. + Moreover, the joyous condition of which laughter is as a: rule the expression is naturally favourable to the general & well- -being of the individual. As will be seen, in WE the various theories of laughter, a remarkable change has taken place in the definitions and associations of the subject. § The extreme emphasis in the earlier accounts of laughter 8 as the expression of derision, scorn and ridicule has been toned down. Laughter, no longer associated with its ugly sister, vulgarity, has become quite respectable, and) joyous laughter, as the expression of happiness, is heartily In the literature of laughter, however, there appears so % many distinguished authorities who have contributed to % : it—many of whom have written remarkably well—that it fi is difficult to imagine that they failed to give a fairly § accurate description of the laughter of the particular & period represented. If so, we are forced to the conclusion that people in earlier times laughed at situations different | from those at which we laugh in the twentieth century. This is not an unreasonable supposition. The quality of k INTRODUCTION 5 laughter has probably improved with a higher and more general standard of education. We have become more refined and find amusement at a higher level of culture. As we have pointed out, we have ceased to laugh at deformi- ties, and many of the humorous elements in early English plays appeal almost exclusively to the younger members of the audience. Moreover, in the study of the sense of humour in children we can trace a marked advance of the type of material which causes laughter as we pass from age to age. It is, thus, natural that there should be a corre- sponding advance in the history of the race. Laughter appears to vary with the social attitude of the period. A fd ARTA AT Px ” The early-Victorian child probably laughed less heartily than the modern child, and, with greater liberty, the range of laughter-provoking elements appears to have increased. In comparing the sense of humour of children of different countries, with a similar standard of civilization, in the present day, we find only minor differences. The favourite funny stories of French, German, American and English children of the same age have much in common. Some stories, undoubtedly of the same origin, stress particular points in the land of their adoption ; others are transplanted unchanged. As a result of an investigation of the sense of humour of coloured children it appears that the humour of the coloured differs widely from that of the white child. Stories which find great favour with the coloured child of twelve years of age are most popular with the white child “# of about the age of nine, and the ratio holds good, approxi- i 1 + W Cad mately, throughout the school period. The appreciation of humorous situations is conditioned ' to a very large extent by the atmosphere, joyous or other- ¢ Wise, in which the appeal is made. To a man suffering "| from seasickness a funny story, which under normal conditions would undoubtedly have provoked laughter, meets with no suitable response. The same is true of children suffering from any physical disability, and the : 6 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER | return to health is not infrequently heralded by a renewed | b: interest in playful humorous activities. On the other 3 hand, the occasion of any release from close attention to 5 an uninteresting task, or the sudden freedom from irksome discipline, is associated with a great sensibility to humorous = appeal. The laughter of children as they come out of a school is a common experience. Much has been written on the origin of laughter. Claims i. have been made for a sense of humour in the more highly ¢ organized animals, and some writers have attempted top trace a correspondence between the muscular movements expressive of pleasure in monkeys and dogs with those of i the laugh of the young child. With the dog the wagging 4 of the tail is the more obvious manifestation of j joy ; ; as als child aptly remarked, ‘a dog laughs with his tail’. But i many dogs, in addition, express pleasure by facial contor- = tions, in which the teeth are bared, resembling a broad § grin, or possibly an incipient laugh. That the young of 1 various species play definite games, which they thoroughly § enjoy, there can be no possible doubt, and in the play- = attitude the existence of a primitive sense of humour can & readily be imagined. In some of Kohler’s experiments on & anthropoid apes, the similarity of emotional response to & that of young children faced with problems of the same 3 type, is very marked. Yerkes’ experiments on chimpan- 2 zees, and his more recent experiments on a gorilla, also i point in the same direction. i The universality of laughter, even among primitive peoples, supports the theory of its instinctive nature. } Here, the incidents which cause laughter correspond | closely with those which cause boisterous amusement among | our young children. Strange dresses; odd sounds; the rough and tumble of incongruous movements; new sensations, represented in the nursery by such experiences | as the appearance of the father wearing the baby’s bonnet, | or the baby with father’s hat on his head, are occasions | INTRODUCTION 7 of great merriment. Tickling has received what appears to be an undue amount of attention by the experts, probably in consequence of its possible connexion with the origin of laughter, and, in these latter days, as a favourite stimulus in the development of a sense of humour in children. That it is a welcome method of bringing about a joyous playful atmosphere is unquestionable. The child laughs at himself which, in itself, is a useful attitude to cultivate. He laughs because so much fuss is being made over an incident which ' causes him so little inconvenience. There is a considerable body of evidence to show that a sense of humour correlates very highly with intelligence. This is recognized by authorities interested in the construc- tion of intelligence tests, which are playing an important role in this country, and still more in America, in educational procedure. In the Report of the Consultative Committee of the Board of Education on ‘ Psychological Tests of Educable Capacity ’, much attention is drawn to the importance of tests based upon the child’s power of recog- nizing absurdities in given statements. The most extrava- . gant example quoted in the Report is the following : ‘ The child is required to read this passage and to discover | as many absurdities as he can : re A SUNDAY IN FRANCE Ten years ago on a pleasant summer’s afternoon in the middle of January, 1916, the twelve o'clock express from Scotland was rushing past the busy terminus of the Great Western Railway at twelve miles an hour. A clean-shaven young Englishman, of about fifty years of age, stepped lightly from one of the first-class carriages and hurried slowly down the platform, with both hands in . his pockets, carrying a heavy bag, and gaily curling the tips of his moustache. There was not a cloud in the sky; and as the rain was | 8 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER | still falling heavily, he took off his mackintosh and strolled out into the crowded streets of the city. The ripening a fields of corn through which he passed were turning golden as the sun set in the south.’ ie The recognized value of a sense of humour leads naturally to the question whether it can be cultivated, and, if so, i what conditions are most favourable for its development. |. As a result of many investigations of the playful activities of children, the author has come to a very definite con- - clusion as to the possibility of such development. The 4 natural difference of inherited bias, such as the tendency of one child to take from the start a joyous interest in life, and another, from the same stock, a more serious outlook, |, is as common as the difference in native ability to be : observed in members of the same family. This need not | concern us here. The important question is whether the I conditions of childhood, the nature of the environment, |. the opportunities offered for freedom of expression, the & frequent contact with other children of a similar age, and so on, are calculated to encourage or discourage the development of a sense of humour. Anything in the nature of rigid discipline, imposed by authority, in which the young child follows out a definite routine is, without doubt, subversive of the development of | humour. The result may be a docile, well-behaved product, but unless the child is endowed with an abnormal bias towards the appreciation of humorous situations, he must inevitably suffer from this type of control. The close | association of playful activities with humour and laughter | points clearly to the necessity and value of a happy and joyous childhood. Periods of great naughtiness, however, should be welcomed on occasions. The child who has | never been really naughty has missed something which is essential to free, natural development, and will probably suffer from this important omission in later life. The § § INTRODUCTION 9 {excessively good child who, according to their statements, i has never given his parents a moment’s anxiety, is generally ifound to be sadly lacking i in a sense of humour. It is a common experience that absence of freedom, and of opportunities for the child to express frankly his own {point of view, have a serious effect on the cultivation of thumour. The difference between children of the same 1social standing, who have attended a school notorious for ithe severity of its discipline on the one hand, and those, “om the other hand, from one where freedom is given to i2xpression within the limits of appropriate discipline, is /hbvious to the most casual observer. Not only is there in ‘he latter group an entirely different standard of humour, ‘out also, what is so frequently associated with it, there is 1 much saner outlook on life. The atmosphere of the ichool has an extraordinary influence on the development ‘Hf humour. id Many of the points which have been lightly touched upon vnere will be dealt with in some detail in later chapters. | CHAPTER II THEORIES OF LAUGHTER (LAUGHTER IN THE SEVENTEENTH AND EIGHTEENTH CENTURIES) ! T is not necessary, in discussing theories of laughter, I to go further back than the seventeenth century. In the treatment of the subject, Thomas Hobbes, in & his remarkably detailed account of laughter, embraces, to 0 a certain extent, the views of Plato and Aristotle, especially as regards the derision theory ; and many other authorities = refer to them. His contemporary, Descartes, apart from & an interesting, but absolutely unconvincing, physiological i description of the act of laughing, is in fairly general accord & with Hobbes in his opinion as to the situations which cause 3 laughter. There is a definiteness in the Hobbes statement which i is most refreshing, and makes it of great value for purposes = of comparison. In this respect it compares very favourably with earlier attempts to explain laughter. The theory of H Hobbes created so much attention, and gave rise to so much in discussion, that it must be quoted in full. He evidently + attached great importance to it, and after the more exhaustive description in the Human Nature section of his work he gave, a year later, a synopsis of it in # Leviathan. The first account is as follows (the italics being retained i as in the original ) ‘ There is a passion that hath no name ; but the sign of 4 it is that distortion of the countenance which we call laughter, which is always joy ; but what joy, what we think, 10 THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 11 and wherein we triumph when we laugh, is not hitherto declared by any. That it consisteth in wit, or, as they call | it, in the jest, experience confuteth : for men laugh at mis- chances and indecencies, wherein there lieth no wit or jest at all. And forasmuch as the same thing is no more ridiculous when it groweth stale or usual, whatsoever it be ¥ that moveth laughter, it must be new and unexpected. Men laugh often, especially such as are greedy of applause from every thing they do well, at their own actions performed never so little beyond their own expectations ; as also at their own fests : and in this case it is manifest, that the passion of laughter proceedeth from a sudden conception of some ability in himself that laugheth. Also men laugh at the infirmities of others, by comparison wherewith their own abilities are set off and illustrated. Also men laugh at jests, the wit whereof always consisteth in the elegant discovering and conveying to our minds some absurdity of another’: and in this case also the passion of laughter proceedeth from the sudden imagination of our own eminency : for what is else the recommending of ourselves to our own good opinion, by comparison with another man’s infirmity or absurdity ? For when a jest is broken upon ourselves, or friends of whose dishonour we participate we never laugh thereat. I may therefore conclude, that. the passion of laughter is nothing else but sudden glory arising from some sudden conception of some eminency in ourselves, by comparison with the infirmity of others, or ‘with our own formerly: for men laugh at the follies of themselves past, when they come suddenly to remembrance, except they bring with them any present dishonour. It is no wonder therefore that men take heinously to be laughed at or derided, that is, triumphed over. Laughter without offence, must be at absurdities and infirmities abstracted from persons, and when all the company may ~ laugh together : for laughing to one’s self putteth all the rest into jealousy and examination of themselves. Besides, + 12 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER / it is vain glory, and an argument of little worth, to think Ia the infirmity of another, sufficient matter for hig triumph.’? The second account gives greater emphasis to special points in the theory and is less argumentative : ‘ Sudden glory, is the passion which maketh those grimaces called LAUGHTER ; and is caused either by some 1 sudden act of their own, that pleaseth them; or by thes apprehension of some deformed thing in another, by coms /parison whereof they suddenly applaud themselves. And»: it is incident most to them, that are conscious of the fewest i= abilities in themselves; who are forced to keep themselves +3 in their own favour, by observing the imperfections of 0 other men. And therefore much laughter at the defects: of others, is a sign of pusillanimity. For of great minds, = one of the proper works is, to help and free others from 1 scorn; and compare themselves only with the most 2 able.’? In tracing the extraordinary changes which have taken place, through the centuries, in the attitude to laughter 2 it is a great advantage to have the standard of the seven- teenth century so explicitly demonstrated in the ‘ Sudden & Glory’ theory of such a distinguished philosopher as Hobbes. Moreover, it held the field against all opponents for a long time and was supported, on different grounds, = by many well-known observers, whose opinions naturally / carried great weight. The critics, on the other hand, © generally satisfied themselves with singling out certain i points for attack without weakening the general thesis to any considerable extent. We must therefore regard the: theory of Hobbes as representating the verdict of the Be seventeenth century on the springs of laughter. The most remarkable feature in the ‘Sudden Glory 1 1 Hobbes, Human Nature. (The English works of Thomas's Hobbes, edited by Sir William Molesworth.) 2 Hobbes, Leviathan. THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 13 theory is that no reference whatever is made to the laughter of children. It deals exclusively with the laughter of grown-up people. Surely, children laughed in the seven- teenth century! The child’s laughter is the real thing, and if only the learned philosophers had given careful attention to it, instead of concentrating on that of compara- tively uninteresting adults, the theory would have had a very short life. The different elements in humour run a natural course through the life of the individual, and to reach satisfactory conclusions it is of the utmost importance that they should be studied in the early stages of their - development. A feeling of superiority is, undoubtedly, a potent stimulus to laughter in children. They are amused, first at the - mistakes of younger children and, later on, at the mistakes i of other people. This is the nearest approach to the sudden | glory element in the Hobbes theory ; the ‘ passion’ being ‘ related to a sense of humour. At the age of seven years ~ this element of superiority, as a cause of laughter, is well i marked. At the age of eight it appears in the front rank, © to the partial exclusion of other springs of laughter. This © 18, par excellence, the time of April-fool jokes, for the making ran April fool of someone else ministers to this feeling of superiority. This corresponds to ‘ that sudden conception of eminency in ourselves ’ referred to in the earlier state- “ment of the theory. It is, of course, true that something | closely connected with this element of superiority may be “used, in abnormal cases, as a weapon in the production of “scorn and derision ; but it is a long cry from the joyous “laughter-provoking activities of childhood to the occasional, “unhealthy products of later years. In other portions of the statement of the theory, the - consideration of the child’s response in laughter to the ‘I different situations referred to, would throw an entirely | different light upon many of the points raised. For ‘ example, the type of joy which finds an outlet in laughter 14 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER i as mentioned in Hobbes’ description, is far removed from: that which gives pleasure to the normal child; and ths stimulus of the new and unexpected is generally of a healthy and wholesome nature. In fact, the ultimate downfall o the ‘ Sudden Glory ’ theory was largely brought about b; applying the acid test of the child’s point of view. In the eighteenth century the most exhaustive, andl probably the most important, contribution to the theory 1 of laughter, was An Essay on Laughter and Ludicyroui Composition, written by Dr. James Beattie, Professor i Moral Philosophy and Logic in the University of Aberdeen He was a very severe critic of the position taken up by Hobbes. He goes so far as to say ‘ The theory of Mr & Hobbes would hardly have deserved notice if Addison had not spoken of it with approbation in the Spectator.’ Aftel quoting a portion of the early statement of the theory 3 Addison had observed that ‘according to this account & when we hear a man laugh excessively, instead of sayings that he is very merry, we ought to tell him, that he is very proud’. Commenting on this Beattie says ‘ It is strange 3 that the elegant author should be aware of this consequence a and yet admit the theory ; for so good a judge of humat nature could not be ignorant, that laughter is not com: i¢ sidered as a sign of pride ; persons of singular gravity beings often suspected of that vice, but great laughers seldom 01» never. When we see a man attentive to the innocentit humours of a merry company, and yet maintain a fixecs solemnity of countenance, is it natural for us to think, that: ; he is the humblest, and the only humble person, in the circle ?’ Similarly, Beattie analyses and severely criticizes | other elements in the Hobbes theory of laughter. 0 In the introduction to his essay Beattie says : i ‘ Of man, it is observed by Homer, that he is the moll wretched, and, by Addison and others, that he is the merriest animal in the whole creation ; and both opinions; are plausible, and both perhaps may be true. If, from the © THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 15 “acuteness and delicacy of his perceptive powers, from his remembrance of the past, and his anticipation of what is to come, from his restless and creative fancy, and from the . various sensibilities of his moral nature, man be exposed to : many evils, both imaginary and real, from which the brutes ‘are exempted, he does also from the same sources derive i innumerable delights, that are far beyond the reach of ‘2very other animal.’ He refers to the laughter of children and distinguishes “between animal pleasures, such as tickling, and sentimental pleasures, such as ideas, and states that the object of his »3ssay is to consider ‘that species of laughter which is at ionce natural and innocent, and to determine what is ‘peculiar to those things which provoke laughter; or, | rather, which raise in the mind that pleasing sentiment or 2motion whereof laughter is the external sign.’ In searching for the springs of laughter Beattie naturally rlays very great stress on the incongruous, as he is dealing ‘particularly with laughter and ‘ludicrous composition ’; - Laughter seems to arise from the view of things incon- r gruous united in the same assemblage ; (1) by juxta-position (2) as cause and effect; (3) by comparison founded on ssimilitude ; or, (4) united so as to exhibit an opposition of meanness and dignity.’ He gives illustrations, at considerable length, of the ruse made by distinguished writers of the different categories in describing humorous situations. In many of the examples quoted more than one element appears as, for example, ‘ We read in the Spectator of a number of men with ‘long chins, whom a wag at Bath invited to dine with him ; and are told, that a great deal of mirth passed on the yoccasion. Here was the collection of incongruities related (10t only by mutual similitude, but also by juxta-position ; Ja circumstance that would naturally heighten the ludicrous isffect’ And again: ‘Incongruous ideas, related by ‘tontiguity, do sometimes acquire a closer connexion, and 16 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER | may become more laughable, when their names being made! equally dependent upon one and the same verb, confer on; it two or more incongruous significations. It is observ: able (says Pope of Prince Eugene), that this General is a great taker of snuff, as well as of towns.” In recent times" Freud in his Wit and its Relation to the Unconscious) develops a similar line of thought, using words with: different meanings, as in the example which he quotes |: ‘one swallow does not make a summer, nor quench the: thirst.’ In the cause and effect section, Beattie gives as a good: illustration of an extremely inadequate combination, in the: case of the tailor of Laputa (Gulliver's Travels), who took the measure for a suit of clothes by means of a quadrant. | / In discussing similitude, which he deals with in some detail fh he says: ‘Laughter often arises from the discovery of» V ! [empectsd likeness between objects apparently dissimilar Y/ and the greater the apparent dissimilitude, and new-# discovered resemblance, the greater will be the surprise: attending the discovery, the more striking the opposition ¢ of contrariety and relation, and the more lively the risiblel: emotion. All men, and all children have a tendency tc: mark resemblances; hence the allegories, similes, and: metaphors, so frequent in common discourse : but readily | to find out similitudes that are not obvious, and were: + never found out before, is no ordinary talent. The persono “& possessed of it is called a man of wit ; especially if at thes! ~_ same time he possess that other talent of conveying hisic /" meaning in concise, perspicuous, and natural language ~* For I agree with Locke, that “ wit consists chiefly in the! assemblage of ideas, and putting those together with quick=:- ness and variety wherein can be found any resemblance oro congruity, thereby to make up pleasant pictures andi: agreeable visions in the fancy ”.’ He points out that many authors in attempting to alter: or improve well-known existing formulas may produce: THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 17 absurdities which cause laughter. As, for example, ‘ It is ‘not uncommon to say of a person who is old, or has long been in danger from a disease supposed mortal, that “ he has one foot in the grave and the other following”. A certain author, speaking of a pious old woman, is willing to adopt this proverbial amplification, but by his efforts to improve it, presents a very laughable idea to his reader when he says, ““ that she had one foot in the grave and the ‘other—among the stars ”.’ Dr. Beattie gives a very scholarly account of the limita- tions of the incongruous as a source of laughter. He insists that it must not be customary and common, and must not excite any powerful emotion such as moral disapprobation, indignation or disgust, pity or fear. He says: ‘May we not, then, set it down as a character of ludicrous absurdity, that it is in some degree new and surprising ? Witticisms that appear to be studied give offence, instead of entertaining ; and nothing sets off a merry tale to so great advantage as an unpromising simplicity of style and manner. By virtue of this negative accomplishment men of moderate talents have been known to contribute more to the mirth of the company, than those will ever do, who, with superior powers of genius were more artful in their language and more animated in their pronun- ciation. Conciseness, too, when we intend a laughable conclusion, is an essential requisite in telling a story; nor should any man attempt to be diffuse in humorous narrative, but he whose wit and eloquence are very great.’ A joke is always the worse for being expected ; the longer it is withheld after we are made too look for it, the more will its volatile spirit lose by evaporation’. And further : ‘Good breeding lays many restraints upon laughter, and upon all other emotions that display themselves externally. And this leads me to refer to those refinements in wit and humour, which take place in society, according as mankind improves in polite behaviour.’ 2 ( \ \ 18 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER In criticizing the opinion expressed by several authors ©» that laughter has a close association with vulgarity he states : ‘ that all men are not equally inclined to laughter ; and that some may be found, who rarely indulge in it | themselves, and actually dislike it in others, cannot be | denied. But they are greatly mistaken, who suppose this character to be the effect of good-breeding, or peculiar to high life. In the cottage you will find it, as well as in the |: drawing-room.’ : In concluding this section of his essay he sums up the | position as follows: ‘If, then, it be asked, what is that | quality in things, which makes them provoke that pleasing emotion or sentiment whereof laughter is the external sign ? I answer, it is an uncommon mixture of relation and contrariety, exhibited, or supposed to be united, in the same assemblage. If again it be asked, whether such a mixture will always produce laughter ? My answer is, it will always, or for the most part, excite the risible emotion, unless when the perception of it is attended with some other emotion of greater authority.’ The variation of the sense of humour with changes in | the social outlook is a matter of great interest, and a special | reference is made to it by the author of the essay, On | Laughter and Ludicrous Composition. He makes an attempt to account for ‘ the superiority of the moderns in ludicrous writing,” and suggests the reasons for it: ‘It seems to be generally acknowledged that the moderns are superior to the ancient Greeks and Romans in every sort of ludicrous writing. If this be indeed the case, it is a fact that deserves the attention of those authors who make wit, or humour, the subject of their enquiry; since the same reasonings that account for this fact must throw light on the philosophy of laughter.’ After discussing the preference of different peoples for their own standard of humour as, for example, that ¢ Shakespeare’s humour will never be adequately relished THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 19 in France, nor that of Moliére in England’, he goes on to say that ‘ ludicrous writing in general is extremely subject to the injuries of time ; and that, therefore, the wit and humour of the ancient Greeks and Romans might have been far more exquisite than we at present have any positive reason to believe.” It is clear, however, that he firmly believes the general opinion to be correct, and gives different reasons to account for, our superiority. Of these the following will suffice : ‘ Religious controversy is in modern times a never-failing source of wit and humour. But in the days of Greece and Rome there was no such thing; the Pagan superstitions being too absurd to admit of controversy. From this source we derive many witty passages in the writings of Chaucer, Erasmus, Pascal, and others; and it is to this we are indebted for Hudibras and the Tale of a Tub, two of the most laughable (I wish I could say the most salutary) pieces of ridicule that ever were written.’ ‘ Gallantry (by which I here understand those generous and respectful attentions we pay to the Fair Sex) contri- butes in many ways both to the copiousness and to the refinement of wit and humour. Nor is there evidence, that this mode of politeness at all subsisted in Greece or Rome, at least in its present form.’ After treating of this at considerable length he remarks : ‘In a word, the superiority, vested by law in the male sex, is now amply compensated to the female, by that tender complaisance, with which they are treated in all polite nations ; and which, from the use they make of it in improving society, and enlivening conversation, it appears that they so justly deserve. Is it not obvious, that this gallantry tends to enlarge the sphere of comic writing ? ‘Neither Aristotle, nor any other ancient, can vie with the moderns, in knowledge of the female character.’ Another cause for our greater variety of humorous experiences, he ascribes to the fact that our customs in - “20 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER regard to dress change more frequently than the Greek or | Roman did. Finally, after dealing with the effect on | ‘ ludicrous writing’ of political institutions, he observes: ‘ And now, notwithstanding the levity of many of these remarks, may we not be permitted to observe in conclusion, that the meek and benevolent spirit of our religion has had a powerful influence in sweetening and refining all the comforts of human society, and conversation among the rest.’ Dr. Beattie’s essay marks a very great advance in the eighteenth century in the sense of humour, as compared | with that of the seventeenth century as represented by Hobbes’s ‘ Sudden Glory’ theory. The encouragement given to laughter by this author was not, however, sup- ported by some of the other distinguished writers of the eighteenth century. Thus, the Earl of Chesterfield in his Letters to His Som expressed himself (Letter 144) very forcibly in his condemnation of laughter : ‘ Having mentioned laughing, I must particularly warn you against it : and I could heartily wish, that you may often be seen to smile, but never heard to laugh while you live. Frequent and loud laughter is the characteristic of folly and ill-manners ; it is the manner in which the mob express their silly joy at silly things ; and they call it being merry. In my mind, there is nothing so illiberal, and so ill-bred, as audible laughter. True wit, or sense, never yet made anybody laugh ; they are above it : they please the mind, and give a cheerfulness to the countenance. . . . I am neither of a melancholy nor a cynical disposition, and am as willing and as apt to be pleased as anybody ; but I am sure that since I have had the full use of my reason, nobody has ever heard me laugh.’ In an earlier letter (129), however, he gives a very useful warning to his son with regard to the evil habit of story telling : ‘ Do not tell stories in company ; there is nothing more tedious and disagreeable ; if by chance you know THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 21 a very short story, and exceedingly applicable to the present subject of conversation, tell it in as few words as possible ; and even then, throw out that you do not love to tell stories, but that the shortness of it tempted you ’. On the other hand, the Earl of Shaftesbury in his essay on The Freedom of Wit and Humour attacks the Hobbes’s theory, and deals with the subject of laughter in an admirably scientific and catholic spirit. Lord Kames, also, in his Elements of Criticism, discusses, at some length, the characteristics and limita- tions of the different elements which cause laughter, and gives an excellent account of its function : ‘ Such is the nature of man, that his powers and faculties are soon blunted by exercise. The returns of sleep, suspending all activity, are not alone sufficient to preserve him in vigour ; during his waking hours, amusement by intervals is requisite to unbend his mind from serious occupation. To that end, nature hath kindly made a provision of many objects which may be distinguished by the epithet of 7isible, because they raise in us a peculiar emotion expressed externally by laughter : that emotion is * pleasant; and, being also mirthful, it most successfully unbends the mind and recruits the spirits. Imagination contributes a part by multiplying such objects without end.’ It would thus appear from the authors quoted, and also from the statements of most of their contemporaries, that laughter in the eighteenth century had become much more respectable than it was in the seventeenth century. The springs of laughter had become less turbid, and a further improvement occurred in the nineteenth and twentieth centuries, with which we shall deal in the next two chapters. CHAPTER III THEORIES OF LAUGHTER (LAUGHTER IN THE NINETEENTH CENTURY) that attachment to any particular view held by previous authoritative writers on the subject, which was so characteristic of the theories of the seventeenth and eighteenth centuries. The effort to establish anything approaching continuity in the development of a consistent theory of laughter was doomed to failure, because of the continual changes in the social outlook, and the nature of the environment, which really conditioned the type of humorous situation which acted as the appropriate stimulus for laughter. There is no longer any necessity for writers in the laughter warfare to declare themselves as disciples of particular schools of thought. The bitter controversies of those fighting under the banners of the Hobbes or the anti-Hobbes factions of the seventeenth century find no similar rivalries, or system of loyalties, to-day. The statement of the view of any special exponent of modern conceptions with regard to laughter is criticised on its merits, and not because of the allegiance of the author to any previous theory. The general discussion of the springs of laughter in the nineteenth century is well illustrated by the contributions of Hazlitt (1819), from the literary point of view, Herbert Spencer (1863), from the physiological side, and Charles Darwin (1872), from that of the most acute observer of natural phenomena the world has ever seen. The con- sideration of opinions expressed by distinguished writers, 22 I the modern theories of laughter there is no longer THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 23 . viewing the subject of laughter from such widely different standpoints, provides an admirable preparation for the appreciation of theories of a more technical and detailed character which will come under review later on. William Hazlitt, in the introduction to his course of lectures on The Comic Writers, etc., of Great Britain, in dealing with the subject of Wit and Humour says : ‘ Man is the only animal that laughs and weeps ; for he is the only animal that is struck with the difference between what things are, and what they ought to be. We weep at what thwarts or exceeds our desires in serious matters : we laugh at what only disappoints our expectations in trifles. We shed tears from sympathy with real and necessary distress ; as we burst into laughter from want of sympathy with that which is unreasonable and unneces- sary, the absurdity of which provokes our spleen or mirth, rather than any serious reflections on it. . . . We turn with an incredulous smile from a story that staggers our belief : and we are ready to split our sides with laughing at an extravagance that sets all common sense and serious concern at defiance.’ In speaking of the normal stress which the serious lays on a given order of events, he defines certain elements of the laughable : ‘ The ludicrous, or comic, is the unexpected loosening or relaxing this stress below its usual pitch of intensity, by ‘such an abrupt transposition of the order of our ideas, as taking the mind unawares, throws it off its guard, startles it into a lively sense of pleasure, and leaves no time nor inclination for painful reflections. The essence of the laughable then is the incongruous, the disconnecting one idea from another, or the jostling of one feeling against another. . . . Humour is the describing the ludicrous as it is in itself; wit is the exposing it, by comparing or contrasting it with something else. Humour is, as it were, the growth of nature and accident ; wit is the product of 24 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER art and fancy. ... The devotion to nonsense, and enthusiasm about trifles is highly affecting as a moral lesson : it is one of the striking weaknesses and greatest happinesses of our nature. That which excites so lively and lasting an interest in itself, even though it should not be wisdom, is not despicable in the sight of reason and humanity.’ As an example of the play upon words, which is such a fertile source of laughter, he gives an account of the author of a periodical called the World, who, looking very worried in church, was asked by a lady what he was thinking about, to which he replied, ‘ the next World.’ He refers to the well-known tendency to laugh in circum- - stances of great gravity : V ‘ It is not an easy matter to preserve decorum in courts of justice. The smallest circumstance that interferes with the solemnity of the proceedings, throws the whole place into an uproar of laughter. People at the point of death often say smart things. Sir Thomas More jested with his executioner. Rabelais and Wycherley both died with a bon-mot in their mouths.’ In common with many writers on laughter, Hazlitt ‘protests against the professional humorist : ‘ An affectation of wit by degrees hardens the heart, and spoils good company and good manners. A perpetual succession of good things puts an end to common conversa- tion. There is no answer to a jest, but another ; and even where the ball can be kept up in this way without ceasing, it tires the patience of the bystanders, and runs the speakers out of breath. Wit is the salt of conversation, not the food.’ Hazlitt’s contribution to the subject of laughter is of great value for the critical exposition, from his great store of material, of the humorous elements in English literature both in his introduction on Wit and Humour and his subsequent lectures. Some of the definitions he gives may THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 25 be called in question, but in his generous outlook on laughter, and by his frank and popular statement of its claims, he cleared away many of the false conceptions of earlier times. X Herbert Spencer, in his comparatively short essay On the Physiology of Laughter successfully demonstrated that a remarkably good case may be made for an explanation of laughter on a physiological basis. Descartes, in his similar attempt to give a physiological account of the laugh, naturally failed in consequence of the somewhat rudi- mentary knowledge of the science of physiology in the seventeenth century. Spencer’s remarkable and scholarly essay marks a great advance in our knowledge of laughter. His singularly lucid statement, with so many definite and clear-cut conclusions, has stood the test of time, and has exerted a great influence on the opinions of many of the writers on laughter since his day. The ambitious problem he proposes to solve in his essay is clearly shown at the beginning : ‘ Why do we smile when a child puts on a man’s hat ? Or what induces us to laugh on reading that the corpulent Gibbon was unable to rise from his knees after making a tender declaration ? The usual reply to such questions is, that laughter results from a perception of incongruity. Even were there not on this reply the obvious criticism that laughter often occurs from extreme pleasure or from mere vivacity, there would still remain the real problem—How comes a sense of the incongruous to be followed by these peculiar bodily actions ? Some have alleged that laughter is due to the pleasure of a relative self-elevation, which we feel on seeing the humiliation of others. But this theory, whatever portion of truth it may contain, is, in the first place, open to the fatal objection, that there are various: humiliations to others which produce in us anything but laughter ; and, in the second place, it does not apply to 26 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER /the many instances in which no one’s dignity is implicated : as when we laugh at a good pun. Moreover, like the other, it is merely a generalization of certain conditions to laughter ; and not an explanation of the odd movements which occur under these conditions. Why, when greatly delighted, or impressed with certain unexpected contrasts of ideas, should there be a contraction of particular facial muscles, and particular muscles of the chest and abdomen ? Such answer to this question as may be possible can be rendered only by physiology.’ Starting with the assumption that nervous excitation always tends to beget muscular motion, he says : ‘If a man receives good news with neither change of feature nor bodily motion, it is inferred that he is not much pleased, or that he has extraordinary self-control—either inference implying that joy almost universally produces contraction of the muscles ; and so, alters the expression or attitude, or both. . . . That laughter is a display of muscular excitement, and so illustrates the general law that feeling passing a certain pitch habitually vents itself, in bodily action, scarcely needs pointing out. It perhaps needs pointing out, however, that strong feeling of almost any kind produces this result. It is not a sense of the ludicrous, only, which does it ; nor are the various forms of joyous emotion the sole additional causes. We have, besides, the sardonic laughter and the hysterical laughter which result from mental distress.’ The part of his essay which has, deservedly, received so much attention, and which stands alone in the literature of laughter, as a satisfactory rational explanation of the act of laughing, is as follows : ‘ It is through the organs of speech that feeling passes into movement with the greatest frequency. The jaws, tongue, and lips are used not only to express strong irrita- tion or gratification ; but that very moderate flow of mental energy which accompanies ordinary conversation, finds a THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 27 its chief vent through this channel. Hence it happens ~ that certain muscles round the mouth, small and easy to move, are the first to contract under pleasurable emotion. The class of muscles which, next after those of articulation are most constantly set in action by feelings of all kinds, are those of respiration. Under pleasurable or painful sensations we breathe more rapidly : possibly as a conse- quence of the increased demand for oxygenated blood. The sensations that accompany exertion also bring on hard-breathing ; which here more evidently responds to the physiological needs. And emotions, too, agreeable and disagreeable, both, at first, excite respiration ; though the last subsequently depress it. That is to say, of the bodily muscles, the respiratory are more constantly implicated than any others in those various acts which our feelings impel us to; and, hence, when there occurs an undirected discharge of nervous energy into the muscular system, it happens that, if the quantity be considerable, it convulses not only certain of the articulatory and vocal muscles, but also those which expel air from the lungs. . . . Thus, then, without contending that the phenomena of laughter in all their details are to be so accounted for we see that in their ensemble they conform to these general principles : that feeling excites to muscular action ; that when the muscular action is unguided by a purpose, the muscles first affected are those which feeling most habitu- ally stimulates; and that as the feeling to be expended increases in quantity, it excites an increasing number of muscles, in a succession determined by the relative frequency with which they respond to the regulated dictates of feeling.’ In developing his fascinating theory of laughter being caused by what he terms descending incongruity, Spencer | 2 bases it on the surplus energy released when a given ~ * situation on the large scale is suddenly replaced by one on a very small scale. The matter appears to be largely i 28 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER mechanical, and the whole is portrayed as in a balance- sheet of income and expenditure. The energy stored to | meet an important event being in excess of that required for the resulting small event, the surplus energy requires | some avenue for its discharge, and the most suitable, and easily available, channel is that which produces the muscular excitation which we know as laughter. As he | describes it : ! “=“Laughter naturally results only when consciousness is | unawares transferred from great things to small—only when | there is what we call a descending incongruity.’ When, however, the final is on a larger scale than the | initial event the balance of income and expenditure is on the wrong side, and there is a shortage of energy to meet the occasion. In this case—ascending incongruity—there is no laughter, and the emotion we call wonder results. In cases of great nervousness far too much energy is stored for simple tasks, and it is a very great relief to be able to work off some of the surplus by muscular movements. | The nervous lecturer who continually changes his position, | unconsciously plays with his watch-chain, cannot keep his | hands in any normal position, and thus releases some of his surplus energy, is a case in point. In this connexion Spencer tells the story narrated by Sir Walter Scott of his school-fellow, who could not say his lesson after the removal | of the waistcoat-button which he was accustomed to finger while in class. Reference is made in the essay to the health-giving function of laughter, in deepening the inspirations and quickening the circulation. Further, for example : ‘ The sudden overflow of an arrested mental excitement, which, as we have seen, results from a descending incon- gruity, must doubtless stimulate not only the muscular system, as we see it does, but also the internal organs ; the heart and stomach must come in for a share of the dis- - charge. And thus there seems to be a good physiological | F THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 29 basis for the popular notion that mirth-creating excitement facilitates digestion.’ . The final blow to the Hobbes theory is given by this essay on the physiology of laughter. It had a far-reaching effect, and a desire to deal with the psychology of laughter on similar lines found expression in Theodor Lipps’ Komik and Humor. Charles Darwin, in The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, makes very significant references to the subject of laughter, and places on a sure foundation many points which are essential to a full understanding of the matter. Some of them have received inadequate, and rather scant, attention by other authorities, though they are of vital importance. It is very interesting to consider the views of Spencer and Darwin together. They do not overlap but they adjust the balance, and give a clearer picture of the whole. With such distinguished and careful researchers, it is natural that particular aspects should receive special attention, and this gives the investigations greater value. Darwin refers to the surplus-energy theory of Spencer with approval and without criticism. In fact, he gives the impression of being of the opinion that, Spencer having come to a definite conclusion on a matter, there is nothing more to be said. In assessing the value of Darwin’s contribution to the origin and characteristics of the act of laughing, it should be borne in mind that the opinions expressed are the result of long investigation and of careful scrutiny of opposing views. An opinion based on actual personal research is in an altogether different category from the opinion of a person, who, with no first-hand knowledge of the subject, satisfies himself with reporting, and possibly criticizing, the results of research. There is much discussion in the present day as to whether laughter is, or is not, of an instinctive nature. Here, of © course, the opinion of Darwin is of supreme importance. 80 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER In referring to the expression of pleasure or joy by monkeys he states : ‘ If a young chimpanzee be tickled—and the armpits are particularly sensitive to tickling, as in the case of our children—a decided chuckling or laughing sound is uttered : though the laughter is sometimes noiseless. Young orangs, when tickled, likewise grin and make a chuckling sound ; and Mr. Martin says that their eyes grow brighter. As soon as their laughter ceases, an expression may be detected passing over their faces, which, as Mr. Wallace remarked to me, may be called a smile.’ And, further: ‘The imagination is sometimes said to be tickled by a ludicrous idea ; and this so-called tickling of the mind is curiously analogous with that of the body. Every one knows how immoderately children laugh, and how their whole bodies are convulsed when they are tickled. The anthropoid apes, as we have seen, likewise utter a reiterated | sound, corresponding with our laughter, when they are tickled, especially under the armpits. | Other vexed questions, such as the nature of laughter and the relation of the smile to the laugh, are admirably | dealt with by Darwin. First as to the sound : ‘ To return to the sounds produced during laughter. We can see in a vague manner how the utterance of sounds of some kind would naturally become associated with a pleasurable state of mind ; for throughout a large part of the animal kingdom vocal or instrumental sounds are employed either as a call or as a charm by one sex for the other. They are also employed as the means for a joyful | meeting between the parents and their offspring, and between the attached members of the same social community.’ ; Then, the relation of the smile to the laugh : ‘ Excessive laughter graduates into moderate laughter. In this latter case the muscles round the eyes are much less s THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 31 contracted, and there is little or no frowning. Between a gentle laugh and a broad smile there is hardly any difference, except that in smiling no reiterated sound is uttered, though a single rather strong expiration, or slight noise—a rudiment of a laugh—may often be heard at the commence- ment of a smile. A smile, therefore, may be said to be the first stage in the development of a laugh. But a different and more probable view may be suggested ; namely, that the habit of uttering loud reiterated sounds from a sense of pleasure, first led to the retraction of the corners of the mouth and of the upper lip, and to the contraction of the orbicular muscles ; and that now, through association and long-continued habit, the same muscles are brought into slight play whenever any cause excites in us a feeling which, if stronger, would have led to laughter ; and the result is a smile.’ In Darwin's observations he clearly traced the develop- ment of the laugh from the smile in his own children ; the laugh being heralded by * a little bleating noise.’ Darwin attaches great importance to the fact that in all parts of the world excessive laughter is accompanied by the production of tears : ‘ With respect to joy, its natural and universal expression is laughter ; and with all the races of man loud laughter leads to the secretion of tears more freely than does any other cause excepting distress. . . . The aborigines of Australia express their emotions freely, and they are described by my correspondents as jumping about and clapping their hands for joy, and as often roaring with laughter. No less than four observers have seen their eyes freely watering on such occasions ; and in one instance | the tears rolled down their cheeks. Mr. Bulmer, a mis- sionary in a remote part of Victoria, remarks, ‘‘ that they . have a keen sense of the ridiculous ; they are excellent { mimics, and when one of them is able to imitate the ' peculiarities of some absent member of the tribe, it is very 32 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER common to hear all in the camp convulsed with laughter.” With Europeans hardly anything excites laughter so easily | as mimicry ; and it is rather curious to find the same fact with the aborigines of Australia, who constitute one of the most distinct races in the world.’ That laughter may be produced by imitation is con- tested : ‘Laura Bridgman, from her blindness and deafness, | could not have acquired any expression through imitation, | yet when a letter from a beloved friend was communicated | to her by gesture-language, she “laughed and clapped her | hands and the colour mounted to her cheeks ”. On other occasions she has been seen to stamp for joy.’ He lays great emphasis on the increased brightness of the eyes during laughter : “A bright and sparkling eye is as characteristic of a pleased or amused state of mind, as is the retraction of the corners of the mouth and upper lip with the wrinkles thus produced.’ On the subject of derision Darwin makes the interesting | comment : ‘In the case of derision, a real or pretended smile or | laugh is often blended with the expression proper to contempt, and this may pass into angry contempt or scorn. | In such cases the meaning of the laugh or smile is to show the offending person that he excites only amusement.’ In summing up the points raised by Darwin the most important are: (1) That laughter is primarily the expres- - sion of joy; (2) that the act of laughing i is accompanied by increased brightness of the eyes, in both monkeys and man ; (3) that there is in monkeys something approaching : laughter, and that it may be caused, as in tickling, by the same means as in the case of children; (4) that, as with ¢ Laura Bridgman, laughter may occur without any possi- - bility of imitation; (5) that there is a common origin of the laugh and the smile ; (6) that laughter, as an expression ¢ THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 33 of joy, is to be found in all peoples ; and (7) that excessive laughter is always accompanied by the secretion of tears. The marvel is that, with such trustworthy evidence, the instinctive nature of laughter should, for so long, have failed to be recognized. CHAPTER IV THEORIES OF LAUGHTER (LAUGHTER IN THE TWENTIETH CENTURY) great debt of gratitude to James Sully, who, in his | Essay on Laughter, in addition to his own valuable investigations on the subject, gives an admirable summary of the views held, with regard to laughter, at the beginning | of the century. In this essay the evidence of the child, at ! long last, receives full recognition. If this had been done earlier, as we have already pointed out, many pitfalls in Qe ce of the history of laughter are under a previous theories might have been avoided. Another welcome departure, in Sully’s survey of the field of laughter, > is the numerous references to the close association of the play-impulse with the appreciation of mirth-provoking activities. He states that the object of his essay is not only to discover the values to be assigned to laughter, but also the appropriate limits to be set to its indulgence. Sully confirms Darwin’s conclusion that there is a series of gradations from the smile to the laugh, that the laugh, in fact, is a grown-up smile. He makes no reference, however, to Darwin's interesting suggestion of the possible priority of the laugh in the course of evolution. In regard to the physiological value of laughter, he is in full agreement with Spencer. The act of laughing is described as a violent interruption of the rhythmic flow of the expiratory movements, and he indicates that the appearance of tears should be regarded as a danger signal, as it is a sign of exhaustion, and shows that the limit has 34 ENE ey Lo ape THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 85 been reached at which laughter is of value hygienically. Relaxation of strain is the universal element in laughter. It results from the collapse of attention, the inhibition energy for attention being set free and resulting in laughter. In the development of laughter, it is interesting to notice the very early age at which natural can be distinguished from artificial laughter. ‘ Preyer tells us he was able to distinguish, in the third year of his boy’s utterances, the genuine laugh of hilarity from that of imitation, which was probably rather more forced.’ Sully, like Darwin, attaches great importance to tickling, in the early stages of development: ‘The experience of being tickled is best described in its entirety as a sensational reflex ; that is to say, a motor reaction on a process of sensory stimulation which produces a well-marked variety of sensation.” The laughter reaction to tickling starts when the child is about two months old if he is mentally alert, but in the case of a less intelligent child it may be a month later. There is an element of the unknown in tickling—you cannot tickle yourself—and the ticklish areas are, as a rule, the parts of the body which are rarely touched. The dressed child is more ticklish than the undressed, and this supports the unknown-element theory. The child’s mind must also be in a pleasurable condition. ‘In order to call forth the glad response of laughter, we must secure a certain adjustment of stimulus to mental attitude. The tickling must fit in with a particular mood, the state of mind which makes enjoyment of fun not only possible but welcome.’ In discussing the varieties of the laughable, Sully gives a comprehensive list of the springs of laughter and deals, in some detail, with the different categories. The ludicrous he defines as that which is a universal cause of laughter. Some of the more important elements which make things laughable, in his opinion, are the following : 36 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER “/ (x) Novelty, out-of-the-way-ness, oddity. The odd is always relative to the custom of a locality or class. (2) Physical deformities. The big nose, the absent chin, obesity, long ears. Additions are more laughable than reductions ; you laugh at the fat man but not at the lean one. + (3) Moral deformities and vices. Drunkenness, violence of temper, vanity, unmasked hypocrisy, stupidity. The amusement varies with the moral standard of the onlooker, the nation or class to which he belongs. (4) Breaches of order and rule. Soldiers marching out of step, tall and short men walking together, and ragging. (5) Small misfortunes. The loss of one’s hat, a fall or slip, sea-sickness, loss of dignity of a vain person. (6) Indecency or obscenity. The coarse-minded person revels in this ‘ muddy spring of laughter’. Unveiling of that which is ordinarily hidden. The coarse joke, however, must have a humorous element or it would not be tolerated. (7) Pretence or make-believe. The unmasking of the imposter or hypocrite. (8) Want of knowledge or skill. Learning to skate or to ride a bicycle. Ignorance or incompetency unmasked in self-assertive people. The naiveté of the child. (9) Riddles, verbal play, and amusing witticisms. The ‘bull ’ is always mirth-provoking, as, for example, ‘ Every , man ought to be ready to give his last guinea to protect "the remainder’. Children’s word-play, puns. Inversion : ‘A mother said to her child, who had been complaining of the heat, “the more you think of it the worse it will be”. Upon which the child remarked, “I should say the worse it is the more I shall think of it.””’ « Most laughter represents the relief from the restraints of ordinary life, and this includes laughter at what is lawless 4 or obscene. In discussing the ludicrous and the relative merits of THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 37 the moral theory or theory of degradation, and the intellec- tual theory or theory of incongruity, Sully, like Spencer, has a parting shot at Hobbes : ‘Is it possible to regard all laughable exhibitions of incongruities as degradations ? Is the charming unsuita- bility of the grown-up’s coat and hat to the childish form viewed by the laughing spectator as a degradation when he ‘lets himself go’ ? Are we laughing at the clothes as degraded by being thus transformed, or at the child’s naiveté as a degradation of human intelligence ? I confess that such a way of interpreting the spectacle strikes me as grotesquely forced.” Whereas, with regard to the alterna- ~ tive he says: ‘The laughter, according to this second en theory, results from a peculiar effect on our intellectual mechanism, such as the nullification of a process of expecta- tion or of an expectant tendency. It is this perfectly disinterested intellectual process which brings about the feeling of the ludicrous and its expression in laughter.’ Sully, however, contends that the main cause of laughter is the novelty of the unexpected. Contrariety to what we are accustomed is a determining element in producing laughter. In many cases there is a combination of the degradation and incongruity theories, and unsuccessful attempts have been made to bring all cases under this combination. In dealing with earlier theories of laughter, the futility of attempting to bring all types of humorous experience into a single category was pointed out. 7 Surprise, followed by a relief from restraint, is responsible for much laughter f from the Bo-peep of infancy through fhe whole school life. Whether laughter has a personal aim, or whether degradation is involved, the superiority element is frequently an important factor. If my friend’s hat blows off I laugh, if my own blows off I donot. There is a great kinship between play and-laughter. In neither is their activity controlled by external ends which have a practical or other serious value. Intellectual laughter may > 88 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER be regarded as mind play. It is of importance to consider primitive and childish laughter in framing theories, if sound conclusions are to be reached. In further discussion of the origin of the laugh, after a full account of Darwin's work, Sully estimates the first appearance of the smile at about the end of the third week and that of the laugh at the end of the third month. As regards the transition from the smile to the laugh, he describes the valuable observations of Millicent Shinn, in The Biography of a Baby, who noticed the wide open mouth five days before the first laugh, which is an open-mouth sound, and which, by reiteration, develops into that of the laugh. The tickling laugh appears at about the same time as the gaiety laugh. As evidence of the dog’s merry ‘ make-believe’, Sully refers to a very interesting account of Professor Lloyd Morgan’s example of this : ‘ The writer tells us that he used at one time to take an intelligent retriever to a sandy shore, where the dog engaged spontaneously in the following pastime. He buried a number of small crabs in the sand, and then stood waiting till a leg or claw appeared, upon which he would run backwards and forwards giving short barks of keen enjoyment.’ Savages delight in teasing and in practical jokes. They laugh much at the acts and customs of the white man, and show great skill in mimicry. The funny man is greatly in request, and there are many savage clowns and jesters. The gaiety is generally of an infantile order. Laughter performs many important functions. It has a great social influence in castigating vices and follies and thus lowering their vitality. It promotes feelings of comradeship and produces an atmosphere of good temper in difficult situations. By ridiculing extremes in social behaviour it has a conservative function. Laughter is a THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 39 . powerful weapon in attacking physical and mental eccentricities and reducing them to the natural order. | Extravagances in dress are also corrected by laughter ; it is a great leveller. Reference has already been made to the variations in the causes of laughter with the mental condition of the particular type of civilization. One test of the stability of progress in social order is, can it with- stand laughter ? A sense of humour is a great solace in / times of stress and strain. ¥ Sully has the great courage to define humour, which he regards as ‘the quieter enjoyment of things laughable’, though he adds, ‘it is difficult to define it scientifically ’. In discussing the difference between wit and humour he states: ‘ Perhaps nowhere do we find the human mind to be more strangely misled by the fact of the existence of two words than in this case. Wit, as essentially a manner of deportment of the intelligence can stand in no simple and direct relation to an emotional mood like humour.’ Sully fears that with an increase of materialism there may be a weakening of the spirit of play, and thus a decline of laughter and mirth. Laughter, he says, ‘is one of the possessions of men to which they should jealously cling. It brings gaiety into what is always tending to grow a dull world. . . . It supplies diversion in youth and still more in age, and it may with a few, as it did with Heine and R. L. Stevenson, remain a bright comrade on the sick-bed. It is the manna on which good fellowship loves to feed. . . . Pure and honest laughter, like mercy, blesses him that gives, and him that takes.’ In his contribution to theories of laughter, Sin Freud confines himself, as indicated by the title of his i book, to ‘ Wit and its relation to the Unconscious ’, and ~ thus covers much less ground than Sully. As one would naturally expect, from such a writer of outstanding | eminence, there are many brilliant suggestions in his work, 40 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER which, from start to finish, is intensely interesting. It is a book, however, which has probably aroused more adverse criticism than any other contribution to the subject of wit and humour. There is much over-elaboration of the obvious, as there is also much under-elaboration of many of the difficult points raised. In some cases it is abnormally difficult to understand exactly what the author means, and, moreover, how some of his conclusions are reached. The stories illustrating the various sections Freud deals with are, on the whole, excellent. Some are very old friends that we are glad to meet again. He prepares us for this in his introduction: ‘We need not shirk the responsibility of analysing the same examples which have already served the classical authors, but we contemplate new material besides to lay a broader foundation for our deductions. It is quite natural that we should select such examples of wit as objects for our investigation as have produced the deepest impression upon our own lives and which have caused us the greatest amount of laughter.’ As a rule, we much prefer the old friends to the new, and there are many of the latter. It adds to the difficulty of following a line of argument if it is over-illustrated ; we cannot see the wood for the trees. Too much analysis of a story should also be avoided. We get very tired, for example, of Heine's amusing ‘ famil- lionaire ’ story, which is ridden to death by over-explana- tion. Whereas, ‘kleptorumania,” ‘anecdotage,’ and ‘ alcoholidays,’” equally good types of condensation, are treated quite naturally. Starting with ‘economy of psychic expenditure '—a ‘modern variant of ‘ brevity is the soul of wit ‘—as the ' all-important cause of laughter is discussed very fully, and illustrated with excellent examples in support of the | thesis. Freud's theory is very closely related to that of Spencer's descending incongruity. Here we meet again the surplus Be THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 41 energy on the credit side of the balance sheet, with its discharge in laughter. In the analysis of the different stories, the material is classified into : Condensation, with mixed word-formation and with modification. The application of the same material, with change of 7 order, slight modification and the same words used in their full or colourless sense. : Double meaning, with name and verbal significance, 7 metaphorical and verbal meaning, true double meaning (play on words), ambiguous meaning and double meaning with allusion. The analyses all pointing in the same direction, namely, that the universal character of wit is to economize. Wit work, according to Freud, must not be confused with understanding work. Nonsense has a technical importance: ° Sense in nonsense transforms. nonsense into wit.’ xcellent examples are given of the play upon words, such as: ‘An undertaker is one who always carries out, what he undertakes ’ and ‘ One swallow does not make a summer, nor quench the thirst’. Some of the conclusions with regard to the inner meaning of stories, however, appear to be far-fetched. For example: ‘A physician, leaving the sick-bed of a wife, whose husband accompanied him, exclaimed doubtfully: “I do not like her looks.” “I have not liked her looks for a long time,” was the quick rejoinder of the husband.” To conclude from this that a state of marital infelicity existed between the man and his wife is absurd ; it might bear a very different construction. The great similarity between wit technique and dream technique is dealt with at some length, but, though very . interesting, the argument does not carry conviction. Freud, in dealing with the tendencies of wit, contributes much of real value. He distinguishes between the purpose- less and purposive : ‘ Sometimes wit is wit for its own sake and serves no other particular purpose ; then, again, it / 42 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER places itself at the service of such a purpose, i.e. it becomes purposive.” As a good example of harmless thought- witticism he quotes: ‘ One man begets the thought, the second acts as its godfather, the third begets children by it, the fourth visits it on its death-bed, and the fifth buries it.” ¥Wit, he defines as ‘ an activity whose purpose is to derive pleasure from the psychic processes’. All pleasures of harmless wit centre round the technique, but it is other- wise with tendency wit. ‘ By belittling and ridiculing our enemy, we obtain the pleasure of his defeat by the laughter of the onlooker.’ The pleasure derived from tendency wit is due to the ' removal of any inhibitions, external or internal, and results | in an economy of psychic expenditure. In harmless wit there is a short circuit of association of words without mental effort, and this also results in a similar economy. Thus, there is an economy pleasure in ‘ the re-discovery of the familiar’. ‘The pleasure in nonsense,” as we may call it for short, ‘ is, in the seriousness of our life, crowded back almost to the vanishing point.” Word pleasure and pleasure in nonsense are, in fact, a relief from critical reasoning. Playing with words and thoughts is, unquestionably, a great pleasure to the child until the age of reason partially stops it. Wit may be senseful or senseless. Reason, critical judgment, and suppression are forces which tendency wit combats in turn. ‘ The psychogenesis of wit has taught us that the pleasure of wit arises from word play or from the liberation of nonsense, and that the sense of wit is meant only to guard this pleasure against its suppression through reasoning.’ 1 Freud agrees with other authorities that laughter, especially with children, is frequently caused by surprise followed by relief from restraint. In referring to laughter as a discharge of energy he says: ‘ Thus, according to our | assumption, the conditions for laughter are such that a sum - THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 48 of psychic energy hitherto employed in the occupation of some paths may experience free discharge. And since not i all laughter (but surely the laughter of wit), is a sign of pleasure, we shall be inclined to refer this pleasure to the release of previously existing static energy.’ One of the most arresting parts of Freud’s contribution to the springs of laughter is that describing the unconscious and the infantile ; witticisms being the result of the action of the unconscious on thought. ‘ The thought which sinks into the unconscious for the purpose of wit-formation only revisits there the old homestead of the former playing with words. The thought is put back for the moment into the infantile state, in order to regain in this way childish pleasure-sources.’ In view of the important part played—according to this thesis—of childish activities in the production of adult wit, it is simply bewildering to read Freud’s reference to child:(_ hood in his concluding summary : ‘It has seemed to us that the pleasure of wit originates from an economy of expenditure in inhibition, of the comic from an economy of expenditure in thought, and of humour from an economy of expenditure in feeling. All three activities of our psychic apparatus derive pleasure from economy. They all strive to bring back from the psychic activity a pleasure which has really been lost in the development of this activity. For the euphoria which we are thus striving to obtain is nothing but a state of a by-gone time in which we were wont to defray our psychic work with slight expenditure. It is the state of our childhood in which we did not know the comic, were incapable of wit, and did not need humour to make us happy.’ In this very imperfect sketch of Freud’s contribution to the theory of laughter an effort has been made to confine it, as far as possible, to his theory of wit-formation. Many of the side-issues raised, however, are very interesting and illuminating. Some of the points on which he is more or A e——— a OS Be \ \ } \ \ 44 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER less in accord with writers, to whom reference has already been made, have simply been noted without discussion. Another very distinguished contributor to our knowledge of laughter also specializes on one department of the subject. - Thus, Henri Bergson, in his book on Laughter, concen- trates on ‘ The meaning of the Comic’. The remarkable variety of opinion expressed by writers on laughter is made more difficult of comparison by the fact that such widely different meanings are attached by them to such words as wit, humour, comic, ludicrous, and so on. If only there were agreed standard definitions of such terms the task would be considerably simplified. Bergson lays special stress on the disciplinary purpose of laughter in correcting extravagant social attitudes. ‘Laughter is a corrective ’, runs through the whole of his interesting essay. His main thesis is that when a living organism behaves, or tends to behave like a machine, laughter is the result. He goes so far as to contend that ‘ the attitude, gestures, and movements of the human body are laughable in exact proportion as that body reminds us of a mere machine’. Thus, when a man falls down in the mud in running for a bus, the reason for our laughter is that he exhibits mechanical inelasticity and clumsiness. The general formula is ‘ something mechanical encrusted on the living causes laughter’. Absent-mindedness, according to Bergson, is one of the great mirth-provoking elements : ‘ It is situated, so to say, on one of the great natural watersheds of laughter.” Absent- mindedness represents a type of mental rigidity, and one of the more important functions of laughter is to prevent it, because people do not like being laughed at. Certain types of deformity tend to cause laughter, but Bergson suggests a limitation: ‘A deformity that may become comic is a deformity that a normally built person could successfully imitate.’ THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 45 Automatism, especially in a ceremonial officer, who performs his duty like a mere machine, is also a fertile source of laughter. Of this Bergson gives two excellent examples : ‘ Twenty years ago, a large steamer was wrecked off the coast at Dieppe. With considerable difficulty some of the passengers were rescued in a boat. A few custom-house officers, who had courageously rushed to their assistance, began by asking them ‘If they had anything to declare.”’ We find something similar, though the idea is a more subtle one, in the remark of an M.P. when questioning the Home Secretary on the morrow of a terrible murder which took place in a railway carriage: ‘The assassin, after despatching his victim, must have got out the wrong side of the train, thereby infringing the Company’s rules.’ Automatism, in fact, represents mental inelasticity and directly mechanization comes in laughter results. Bergson deals admirably with another cause of laughter, which involves the relation of physical to spiritual values ; ‘Any incident is comic that calls our attention to the physical in a person, when it is the moral side that is concerned.” Whenever the transition of characters associa- ted with those of the body to those of the soul, or vice versa, is clumsily and rapidly effected it causes great merri- ment, as, for example, when a man sneezes during a pathetic oration, or when a man sits down during an eloquent peroration, and thus draws attention from the soul to the body. Bergson quotes a case in which, at a funeral oration, the deceased was referred to as ‘ virtuous and plump ’. Whenever a man is embarrassed by his body it tends to cause amusement. This is suggested as the reason why the fat man or woman nearly always appears in funny stories told by children. Probably, however, the deformity element also enters into it. To summarize the evidence ‘of the theory of laughter being caused by something 46 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER mechanical being encrusted on the living, Bergson says : ‘ We laugh every time a person gives us the impression of being a thing.’ Whenever a clever arrangement is constructed, by which inanimate objects are made to behave as if they were alive, laughter is caused. Marionnettes are a continual source of joy to the child, and the same is true of the Jack-in-the- box and the Punch and Judy show. The adult also delights in these things if he has retained an appropriate measure of the spirit of the child. This largely determines the appreciation of the comic by the grown-up. We do not sufficiently recognize the importance of the childish element in most of our joyful emotions. In passing from these inanimate representations of life to the comedy we simply replace material things by ideas. Bergson deals in some detail with repetition and inversion —as when the prisoner lectures the judge and the child teaches his mother—as fertile sources of laughter. In speaking of the amusement caused by fitting an absurd idea into a well-established formula, the case is quoted of the lazy man who said ‘I don’t like working between meals’ in place of ‘ I don’t like eating between meals’. Similarly, ‘A comic effect is obtained whenever we pretend to take literally an expression which was used figuratively.’ Thus, when a mother speaks to her son of the evils of gambling and says, ‘ It is very risky; you win one day and lose the next’, and the boy replies, * Well then, I will gamble only every other day’, it causes amusement, because the boy has taken a remark literally which was used figuratively. Many interesting examples of childish naiveté are due to the inability of the child to distinguish between the literal and the figurative. Bergson cannot free himself from the overruling concep- tion that the great function of laughter is that of stern discipline. At one point in describing the comic character he goes so far as to say : ‘ Here, too, our first impulse is to THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 47 accept the invitation to take it easy. For a short time, at all events, we join in the game. And that relieves us from the strain of living.” Then comes the depressing addition : ‘ But we rest only for a short time. The sympathy that is capable of entering into the impression of the comic is a very fleeting one. It also comes from a lapse in attention. In other words, it is a form of absent-mindedness, and must be condemned.’ His conclusion would have rejoiced the heart of Hobbes : ‘ Laughter is, above all, a corrective. Being intended to humiliate, it must make a painful impression on the person against whom it is directed. By laughter, society avenges itself for the liberties taken with it. It would fail in its object if it bore the stamp of sympathy or kindness.’ With so much that is delightful and entertaining in this charming essay, we can only regret that more attention has not been given to the joyous laughter of children, and the generally accepted opinion of other modern contributors that laughter, quite apart from its use for the correction of social extravagances, is of priceless value in other far more important directions. In this connexion, it is interesting to compare Sully’s estimate of the function of laughter with that of Bergson. They are as far apart as the poles. Boris Sidis; in The Psychology of Laughter, like Sully, |. deals with the whole subject of laughter and, incidentally, criticizes very vigorously the conclusions of Freud and! Bergson. Economy in psychic expenditure as a cause of 3 laughter is, to this authority, anathema. To him, the =} great stores of energy, in process of release, produce that/ * feeling of relief which finds expression in laughter. W laugh from strength not from weakness. ° Laughter comes not out of economy, but out of abundance. . . . Conscious: ness of reserve energy gives rise to joy and merriment with ~ their concomitant manifestations of smiling and laughing, v 48 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER Whenever and wherever a stimulus can tap a source of reserve energy which is mentally experienced as an abun- dance, joy and laughter come to life. There is no economy and no niggardliness in the source of laughter. Laughter is born of lavishness and dies with thriftiness.’ Some of the ‘criticisms of Freud go too far in concentra- ting, here and there, on unfortunate expressions in Wit and its Relation to the Unconscious, without fully appreciating the evidence on which the statements are based. Thus, the Freudian economy frequently results, as we have seen, in the discharge of the energy stored for inhibition purposes in both the conscious and the unconscious. The formula “brevity is the soul of wit’, which should not be unduly emphasized, assumes an importance which is almost of the nature of an obsession, and this has led to much confusion. To take shelter behind a formula which is only a general expression, and which does not rest upon a scientific basis, is inimical to clear thinking. Wit he defines as ‘ that form of thought and expression which give rise to the free, spontaneous, mental activity, due to the arousal of subconscious reserve energy.’ Perhaps the most valuable element in the contribution of Sidis to the theories of laughter is his insistence on the close association of laughter with the play impulse. ‘The play instinct must remain dominant. We laugh in play. . . . The energy spent in laughter should be felt as not tending to any useful purpose. It must be spent for its own sake, for the love of it. . . . The laughter of triumph runs through all the stages of life.” He goes so far as to contend that the source of all laughter is the release of reserve energy. ‘ If we remember that play is a manifesta- tion of spontaneous, unrestrained activity we can begin to understand the nature of laughter, which is one of the manifestations of the play instinct, present, not only in man, but in the whole animal world.’ In common with other observers, Sidis notes that the : THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 49 act of being relieved from any great strain generally results in laughter, and also that what is not customary is laughed at. He also stresses the delight we take in discovering the mistakes made by people in high official positions : ‘ Whatever reveals weakness in an object of superior standing and releases in the audienee subconscious sources of hidden reserve energy is a fit subject for laughter and ridicule.” Sidis, like Sully, summarizes the variety of humorous experiences which cause laughter by a series of comparisons : ‘ The significant and the insignificant, the - noble and the ignoble, the grave and the gay, the heroic and the grotesque, the unusual and the usual, the superior and the inferior, when juxtaposed, raise laughter.’ In dealing with the ludicrous, Sidis starts with the assumption that it has its origin in stupidity of one kind or another, and that, whereas crime and sin are punished by law and religion, stupidity is chastened by laughter. ‘To laugh at the infirmities of human nature, to prick social, moral, religious, and family bubbles and see them explode will ever remain the joy and the essence of the ludicrous.’ In a very large number of illustrations, chiefly of Irish descent, Sidis shows what an extraordinary interest is taken in smart repartee in accounts of humorous experiences. It certainly is the predominant laughter- provoking element in a vast number of good stories. It brings pleasure to the successful wag and to the onlooker. In the cases quoted there is a notable absence of derision, though the superiority of the triumphing opponent is manifest. Throughout the book the release of energy is the mainspring of laughter. Anything in the nature of malice or cruelty tends to fall into disuse, and is eventually completely eliminated, with a more cultured outlook in social intercourse. Thus, ‘ If we come to analyse the comic we find that its object is the awakening of the subconscious surplus energy of man, bringing to the foreground the play 4 50 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER of free, unimpeded activity, giving rise to pure joy, resulting in laughter’. The function of laughter, as described by Sidis, is not unlike that of Sully, and is in striking contrast to that of Bergson. ‘In the darkness of man’s life laughter is the light of the spirit. . . . The highest point reached by laughter is intimately related with the highest intellectual aesthetic, and moral development.’ Sidis does not reach the point of declaring laughter to be an instinct, but he comes very near to it in linking it up so definitely with the play impulse, which he assumes to be. an instinct. He again comes very near when he compares | it with the fighting instinct : ‘ Laughter and the fighting instinct are akin only in so far as both of them are mani- festations of superabundant energy.’ Although he fails to arrive at this conclusion, the whole of his detailed survey of laughter would fit in admirably with the instinct theory. William McDougall, in An Outline of Psychology, gives a very interesting account of a new theory of laughter. He is of opinion that the smile and the laugh are not| definitely connected in origin, as suggested by Darwin, ~ Sully and other writers. He does not regard the laugh as a grown-up smile. In treating of laughter caused by the minor misfortunes of others, he concludes that as a safe- guard to what he terms ‘ primitive passive sympathy ’, 1ti is necessary to have some alternative to sympathy in cases of this kind. Laughter is that alternative. In other: words, ‘ we laugh that we may not weep’. ‘ We are saved from this multitude of small sympathetic pains and depres-: sions by laughter, which, as we have seen, breaks up out train of mental activity, prevents our dwelling upon the distressing situation, and provides an antidote to the depressing influence in the form of physiological stimulatior: that raises the blood pressure and promotes the circulation: of the blood. This, then, is the biological function oi THEORIES OF LAUGHTER 51 laughter, one of the most delicate and beautiful of all Nature’s adjustments.” In fact, he sums up the position by saying : ‘ The true theory of laughter may be stated in one sentence—laughter is the antidote to sympathy.’ McDougall “attaches considerable importance to the subject of tickling in the history of laughter, and suggests that ‘ laughter on being tickled is the crudest and earliest - form of humour’. In admitting laughter to the group of minor instincts he says: ‘We must recognize that it differs from all other instincts in that its impulse seeks no goal beyond itself, but secures its own satisfaction by means of bodily processes which effect nothing in the outer world.’ : Max Eastman, in his admirable book on The Sense of Humour, deals in a scholarly and very lucid manner with the important question of the instinctive nature of laughter, in support of which he is an excellent and convincing exponent. After discussing the tests for instinct as elaborated by McDougall, and the important addition by James Drever—‘so early and so spontaneously '—and showing how laughter fulfils all the necessary requirements, he makes out a case for its instinctive nature which is unanswerable. Among the many good arguments he adduces in discussing the history of laughter, in favour of its instinctive basis, Eastman makes an important point which does not appear to have received sufficient attention from other observers : ‘ I have been very careful to observe that when I hear or see a group of people laughing heartily at some funny tale or incident, although I have not heard or seen the thing at which they laugh, my own response is not only laughter but humorous emotion. And this fact completely refutes the theory of Lipps and Spencer, that humorous laughter is a mechanical result of a passage of attention over a defined series of objects or ideas. It refutes also the theory that such laughter is a mere sign 52 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER of release, for no matter what one’s own condition when he notices that others are enjoying a joke—whether he is tense with enterprise or some attempt at propriety, or whether he is relaxed and perhaps half asleep in a railroad berth—that subtle and incomparable enjoyment steals through him just the same. Humorous laughter is infec- tious—and that alone is sufficient to establish it among the other instinctive adjustments of mankind to his environ- ment.’ CHAPTER V LAUGHTER AND DREAMS it is interesting to observe how many authorities trace a very definite resemblance between the arrange- ment of the material in humorous situations and that which can be noticed in the records of dreams. The conditions of the dream, with the complete absence of anything in the nature of critical reasoning, are obviously favourable to freedom for an extended use of absurd combinations in the bringing together of a great variety of previous experiences, not in their original connexions but in a new orientation, to meet the purposes of the dream. Many of these amusing combinations found in the dream would have been impos- sible in the waking condition without the very rare gift of an imagination capable of freeing itself entirely from the chains of conventional methods of grouping experiences. Bergson only touches lightly on the subject of laughter and dreams. He does not attach so much importance to the dream as Freud, but it helps to illuminate his ideas of the comic. He regards the state of dreams as a sane condition of mind which resembles madness, and with the same association of ideas as is to be found in lunacy, and he concludes that, ‘ comic absurdity is of the same nature as dreams’. He points out that in the dream we can analyse sense impressions received from the outside world, and use those elements necessary to satisfy the mood of the dreamer, and the idea that fills his imagination at the time. Thus, “ a gust of wind blowing down the chimney becomes the howl of a wild beast or a tuneful melody’. There are certain situations in the dream which, due 53 I the literature associated with the springs of laughter 54 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER to the relaxation of reasoning, would not occur anywhere but in dreamland, and this gives the dreamer a distinct advantage over the humorist. Bergson emphasizes this interesting point: ‘But above all, there is a special madness that is peculiar to dreams. There are certain special contradictions so natural to the imagination of a dreamer, and so absurd to the reason of a man wide-awake, that it would be impossible to give a full and correct idea of their nature to any one who has not experienced them.’ In this clear summary we have a vivid comparison of the relation of the dream to the normal development of humorous situations. Children derive great pleasure from the recollection of incidents of dreams, and the impossible events which arouse fairyland elements of special interest. Thus, the young child who dreams that she is married to an officer and puts her children in a place of safety during an exciting experience, delights in remembering events which cause no amusement in the dream but produce hearty laughter in the waking state. The spirit of wild adventure in the performance of heroic deeds finds full expression in the dream, and the comparison of such joyous experiences with the uninteresting events of normal life appeals” to the sense of humour of the child. The sense of time and distance is obliterated ; the dreamer suddenly finds him- self in a different part of the world and as suddenly finds himself at home. All this adds to the wonder and charm of the dream. Freud deals somewhat exhaustively with the connexion of wit-work with dream-work, and is, at times, somewhat difficult to follow. He attempts to apply the dream- technique with its condensation, displacement, and indirect expression to wit-technique. In this he is only partially successful and he appears to recognize it. There are, of course, similar elements, as is generally recognized, in the development of the dream and of wit. He says, however, - LAUGHTER AND DREAMS 55 quite frankly : ‘I really do not trust myself to make the assertion that I have succeeded in explaining all the current witticisms with reference to their techniques, and I there- fore admit the possibility that my enumeration of wit- techniques may show many gaps.’ We have already discussed his references to the unconscious and the infantile in wit-formation. In witticisms that serve unconscious tendencies he appears to be on comparatively safe ground. Freud admits, however, that ‘in the case of tendency-wit of other varieties, namely, harmless wit and the jest, this power seems to fall away, and the relation of the wit to the unconscious is an open question’. In summihg up the whole question he says: ‘I earnestly wish that it were possible for me on the one hand to present one decisive point in my conception of wit more clearly, and on the other hand to fortify it with compelling arguments. But as a matter of fact it is not a question here of two failures, but of one and the same failure. I can give no clearer exposi- tion because I have no further testimony on behalf of my conception.” There is so much of interest in his work on wit and the dream that it is hoped he may, later on, develop important points which, he is the first to admit, need further elaboration. Boris Sidis only refers incidentally to the relation of laughter to the dream, but the reference is interesting : We can compare the comic and laughter with rest. In fact, we may go further and compare laughter with sleep ; not with the sleep in which the senses and consciousness are inactive, but with the sleep state in which mental activities are present. May we not compare the ludicrous with the dream ? The dream occurs during the rest state, during sleep. And what is sleep but a release from all the troubles and trammels of waking life 7° Laughter is so dependent on a satisfactory environment for its expression that the dream or rest, which relaxes the strain of serious attention, and is foreign to a conventional attitude of mind, gives an 56 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER opportunity for the development of situations the remem- brance of which causes laughter. Normal healthy children are very rarely seen to laugh in their dreams, though they frequently smile, probably as a result of the pleasure derived from wish-fulfilment rather than the appreciation of any humorous element, which is recognized as humorous, in the dream state. The recogni- tion of the absurd combination of selected material needs the reasoning power of the waking state to enjoy the incongruity of the juxtaposition of the factors employed in the construction of the dream. In dreaming, the rapid switching off of one line of thought to another plays an important part, and gives rise to many ridiculous situations. Thus, a child describing a dream involving a harrowing account of the funeral of her brother interposes, in the middle of the narrative, a school experience in which her sums are being marked by her teacher. From an investigation of a very large number of child- ren’s dreams the author has rarely come across a case in which the dreamer recognizes anything absurd in the most incongruous grouping of material. The accounts of dreams give no record of laughter at comical incidents, though there may be a wealth of suitable material for the purpose. This is in keeping with the observation of sleeping children referred to above. In other words, the child does not see jokes nor recognize funny incidents as such in his dreams. Any evidence to the contrary is probably due to a confusion of the hypnagogic, or half-waking state with that of true sleep. In this connexion the records of dreams are more reliable than the observation of sleepers. The following examples from a collection of children’s dreams ! are sufficient for the purpose of showing the non- recognition by the dreamers of the absurdity of glaring incongruities : (a) A boy of 5 years of age: ‘A burglar came into my ! Kimmins, Children's Dreams. LAUGHTER AND DREAMS 57 house and stole mother’s money. He stuck a knife into me, and I dreamt that he had shot another boy after he had killed me.’ (6) A boy of 8 years: ‘I dreamt that I was going to be washed. And then I was being put in the bath to be washed. After I was washed, I was rung out in the mangle. Then I was hung on the line. I was hanging on the line when it started to rain. My mother took me in and ironed me. The iron was hot. And then I woke up.’ (c) A girl of 15 years: ‘I was seated in a boot-shop, which somehow seemed something like our kitchen, and yet it had all the fittings of a shop where new boots were sold, and others mended. My father and mother were both there, and I was asking to see some fancy indoor shoes. The first pair the man brought me were of black glacé kid, with patent leather toe-caps and high heels. They were similar to what are called court shoes, having no straps. I put them on and they fitted me perfectly, so I asked the price of them, and was very much astonished to hear that they were five pounds four and sixpence. I told the shopkeeper that they were too much and enquired if he had something a little cheaper. He then brought a pair of rather shabby black brocaded ones, also with high heels. These also fitted me, and again I asked the price, which went to the opposite extreme, for they were only twopence-halfpenny. I do not know whether I had these or not, for my dream suddenly ended.’ There is no indication in these records of amusement at any of the incidents of the dreams, and the only trace of reasoning is the surprise of the girl at the price of the shoes. There is the same absence of amusement and laughter in the dreams of American children. Here is an example of an incongruous fear dream: ‘Once I dreamt that I was captured by cannon bals [evidently cannibals]. They all began to jump and yell. I was surprised to see myself in 58 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER my own parlour. There was a fire, and a kettle was over it full of boiling water. They threw me into it and once in a while the cook used to come over and stick a fork into me to see if I was cooked. Then he took me out and gave me to the chief, who was just going to bite me when I woke up.’ Although, as we have seen, there is practically no impression of incongruity, and its expression in laughter, in the dream, we find in the dreams of early adolescence that sorrow does find expression in tears. Thus, in dreams in which there is a well-marked sad element, it is not unusual to find at the end of the record some such expres- sion as ‘ and when I woke up I found that my pillow was wet with tears’. The dream with its great range of variety of grouping of experiences is in itself a relaxation from the normal routine of daily life, and gives the pleasure which we associate with the fairy story. The relation of cause to effect does not _ trouble the dreamer, and he accepts, without question, the arrangement of the visual and auditory material presented to him, however incongruous it may be, CHAPTER VI LAUGHTER AND TEARS loved without recognizing each other, and there was born to them a son, who was Humour.” The remarkable alliance between laughter and tears is well known. It manifests itself in very early life. The crying baby, if the cry is not due to actual pain, can easily have ~ his attention diverted and the transfer from tears to laughter effected. Sunshine and showers alternate rapidly in the world of the child of unstable type. As a rule, the neurotic child laughs at the slightest provocation, and cries with equal readiness. On the other hand, the stolid complacent child who rarely laughs does not frequently indulge in tears. In later life, in cases of acute hysteria, the bewildering and rapid passage from laughter to tears or in the reverse direction, is characteristic of the abnormal mental state. The kinship of the emotional states, of which laughter and tears are the outward and visible signs, is fundamental. At times of great stress and strain it is often a question as to which method of relief—laughter or tears—will be most effective in getting rid of, so to speak, the waste products of the released inhibitions of an emotional storm. In a somewhat incongruous manner, the representatives of joy and sorrow frequently join in the common task, and thus demonstrate effectively their close relationship. It is a not uncommon practice for intimate friends, who have taken part in the sad obsequies of a funeral of some dearly loved relative, to partake of a meal together after the ceremony. Up to this point tears have been the prevailing 59 J- and Grief, meeting in the nocturnal forest, 60 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER element, and have served a valuable purpose in securing a method of relief from the profound emotional strain. Reminiscences are, naturally, now freely interchanged, and if, by chance, some incident referred to has a slightly humorous bearing, it is received with uncontrolled laughter, out of all proportion to its real value as a mirth-provoking contribution. Laughter has taken the place of tears, and performs the most important function of providing additional relief to the emotional burden of a great SOITOW. In the London air-raids the children who cried at the terrifying experiences, and so obtained relief by tears, suffered very little; and those who found an outlet by great activity and occasional laughter slept well, and appeared to suffer no evil consequences. It was those who suppressed their emotion, and were too proud to confess that they felt any fear, with whom the result of the terrible incidents of those evil days remained for some time as a serious handicap. The value of laughter and tears in working off undue emotional disturbances cannot be over- estimated, and, when they occasionally act in conjunction, the relief may be considerably increased. A good example of the united action of laughter and tears has already been referred to, namely, that prolonged laughter leads to exhaustion, and the lachrymal glands are then affected, with the production of tears, which increase the relief. Darwin, as we have seen, was particularly interested in this strange consequence of excessive laughter, and found that even among primitive peoples the curious conjunction of laughter and tears was a common experience. The susceptibility to a rapid transfer from the expression of one to that of the other is therefore by no means confined to children, and, moreover, it is not restricted to the more highly civilized races. It is a matter of such absorbing interest that it would reward fuller investigation, as it appears to open up a fascinating field for further research. LAUGHTER AND TEARS 61 Incidentally, it bears upon the question of the instinctive nature of laughter. In the contemplation of the minor misfortunes of others, we are frequently faced with the important question, ‘ Shall we laugh or shall we weep?’ Does the appropriate solution of a situation suggest laughter or tears ? Humour points in one direction, sympathy in the other. This problem, as we have seen, has been the bone of contention, in the varying attitude among the authorities, in the conflict between different theories of laughter. It is intimately bound up with the respectability of laughter, and, to a large extent, with the respectability of tears. If laughter is vulgar, and its employment the sign of an untutored mind, its companion, tears, must also come under the ban. An abnormal use in public of any expres- sion of such emotional feeling as is represented by laughter or tears comes under the general condemnation of what is popularly known as ‘ bad form’. This was, to a certain extent, the verdict of the seventeenth century on the matter. The abuse of laughter and tears, by excessive display on one side or the other, undoubtedly merits disapproval. Giggling is probably the most exasperating form of the degradation of laughter. It is, almost exclusively, a feminine vice, and attains its maximum development in the earlier stages of the adolescent period. The giggling girl, as a rule, also indulges readily and frequently in tears. The transfer from giggling to weeping is as common as the change from laughter to tears in the very young child. Giggling, if continued into later life, may be regarded as a sign of mental defect, and is frequently associated with a well-marked hysterical tendency. Fortunately, however, the giggling stage is not, under favourable conditions, of long duration. This peculiarly irritating form of laughter disappears as suddenly as it came into existence, and, with it, the easily aroused lachrymose bias. Laughter and tears 62 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER thus appear to remain associated even when they depart from a normal standard of respectability. From this apparent digression we return to laughter and tears under ordinary conditions. It is not suggested that humour is devoid of an element of sympathy; on the contrary, it is one of its characteristics. The capacity for taking a wide view of a situation, and detecting in it a factor of comic interest, would be severely limited if no quality of a sympathetic nature were present in its constitu- tion. In dealing with the misfortunes of others, the question of the dominance of that which turns the scale in favour of laughter, as against that which results in favour of tears, is of the greatest importance. Take the oft-quoted case of the man whose hat is blown off by the wind. Provided always that the hat belongs to some one else there is no doubt as to the decision. We laugh because, in spite of any slight sympathetic feeling which may exist with regard to the temporary loss and inconvenience to the person concerned, the comic element dominates the incident, and there is no place for tears, nor for any over- ruling sympathetic interest to induce us to suppress our laughter. : The position, however, may be reversed by a slight alteration of the conditions. A boy is looking over a bridge, and a sudden gust of wind blows his hat into the river, and there is practically no chance of its recovery. No one laughs. Our sympathy is at once aroused—especially if he is a poor boy—and may find expression in the allevia- tion of his distress by compensation for his loss. There are borderline cases in which the two elements are fairly evenly balanced, and the resulting laughter or tears will largely depend upon the social attitude of the onlooker. Young children are frequently—quite wrongly—harshly criticized on account of their actions on such occasions. The play impulse is such a controlling influence in early life § that the decision in favour of laughter of the five-year-old - LAUGHTER AND TEARS 68 may conflict with the more mature judgment of the tén-year-old child. In the laughter of primitive peoples the misfortunes of others play a much more important part than other elements of humour, and even the struggles of a drowning man are occasionally the cause of much merriment. In the record of favourite funny stories and jokes of young children, much interest is concentrated on the misfortunes of others at the ages of 7, 8, and 9 years. Then, they rapidly decline in favour, and are replaced by those involv- ing other types of humorous situations. Throughout the series the appeal of this kind of humour is much stronger with the boys than the girls. - The girls prefer fairy and domestic stories. In the borderline cases referred to the effect produced on the boys may be totally different from that on the girls as the result of the more sympathetic nature of the latter. Although the attitude towards the misfortunes of others shows more effectively the relations of laughter to tears, the connexion is by no means confined to this particular type of humorous appeal. The relief from strained attention, necessitated by a variety of causes, may be in the direction of laughter or tears, or, as we have found, in a combination of both. The joyous laughter at church parade is a common experience. For some time we have been in a position in which laughter or tears were impossible, or, at any rate, would have been unbecoming. Our emotions have been stirred, possibly by a powerful appeal from the pulpit, by the singing of a favourite hymn, or some other element in a devotional service. Tears, in such circumstances, are nearer the surface than laughter. Then comes the opportunity for relief, and laughter serves the purpose of the release of the consequences of conflicting emotions. ; Some great anxiety clouds our sky. We are overwhelmed with grief, and find solace in tears. Suddenly, we receive 64 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER the joyous news ; the lost is found, or the child has survived the dangerous operation and is on the high road to recovery, and laughter and tears combine to relieve the mental strain. This alternation of laughter and tears is not an unusual experience in periods of exciting crises. It is not sufficiently recognized that in the process of emotional development towards the final result of laughter or tears there is, in every stage, a well-marked tendency to transfer. This is clearly shown in the case of an exciting narrative, in connexion with which we are wondering whether the story will have a joyous or a sorrowful ending. If it points, near the end of the story, to a tearful conclusion, any subsidiary comical incident produces an effect far beyond its intrinsic merits. That is, in the neighbourhood of tears, comic interest finds a particularly favourable market. With obvious limitations, the converse may be equally effective. The play-writer, if he is to obtain success, must secure a certain emotional balance—a proper relationship between laughter and tears. There must be, in the most serious play, a certain amount of comic relief, and, on the other side, a long succession of laughter- provoking incidents produces a feeling of boredom, even to the least intelligent of the groundlings. There must be an appropriate admixture of light and shade. What is contemptuously known, in theatrical representa- tions, as sob-stuff, will always play a not unimportant part in the structure of important plays of general appeal. In analysing any really successful play or novel, it is not difficult for the most casual observer to discover the sob- stuff element. Characters and situations, apparently unnecessary for the structure of the play or narrative, are deliberately introduced to secure the necessary emotional bias without which the work would have failed to obtain popularity. Itisa distinct advantage for there to be a back- ground of laughter for tears, and a background of tears for laughter, by whatever terms they are designated. In LAUGHTER AND TEARS 65 obtaining the appropriate emotional setting, external factors may render important service. The scenery of the theatre or the film, and the cinema orchestra—with a selection of music calculated to enhance the emotional effect of the visual appeal—all assist to produce the necessary environment. The laughter or tears of the mob attest the infectious nature of joy and sorrow. The standards attained by the group generally fall below those of the individual. In fact, as members of a group, we frequently laugh at situations of which we heartily dissapprove under ordinary conditions. We can, also be moved to tears by mass suggestion at incidents which would leave us unmoved in private life. Much of the adverse criticism of laughter is based on the observation of the behaviour of the mob in an emotional state. The harsh—sometimes cruel—laugh of derision of the excited crowd, which has for its object the holding up to ridicule of opponents, leaves little opportunity for the I expression of tolerant judgment, which it is one of the main ~ functions of humour to achieve. In many disputes, the humorous suggestion is one of the most potent factors in arriving at a peaceful solution, and the restoration of harmony. The tendency of laughter is generally to act the role of a peacemaker, not that of a provoker of quarrels. The intimate association of laughter with the play impulse, as seen in the joyous merriment of children, continues long after the stage of childhood is passed. Laughter does not change its nature with advanc- ing years. We may laugh less with a diminishing play impulse, but, as we shall see in the next chapter, we all retain something of the spirit of the child, and with this we cling to laughter as of old. The great value to tears of the companionship of laughter is due to its power to revive that common experience of association with the play impulse which has been temporarily eclipsed by grief. There are other reasons why laughter should be a welcome 5 66 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER guest in the house of mourning. The laughter at the funeral feast was no insult to the memory of the one whose loss we mourned. The sadness of the expression of sorrow in tears is, that though they afford relief, there is an element | | 1 of futility in the method. There is no building process, no restoration. Laughter restores in that it re-educates; it brings us once more into contact with the pure, temporarily | forgotten, joys of childhood. Moreover, it brings a message of health. However much we may differ as to its psycho- logical content, there is no doubt as to the physiological i value of laughter. We are strengthened, and the improve- | ment in health gives an added zest to life, with the result | that we may be prepared to make a fresh start. We have seen something of the infectiousness of laughter and tears when the herd instinct dominates the group. In more intimate gatherings the infectious character of laughter is also in evidence. It tends to tone down any rough places in dissension, and, by its sympathetic character, discloses unrecognized points of agreement. It affects the nature of the witticism, and influences the | - attitude taken to a humorous situation. * Laughter is sometimes described as the antidote of grief, but we have found that there is a closer relationship, and that, at times of great strain, laughter and tears come to each other’s relief. The quaint story, already referred to, Ar of the origin of humour is not without interest. CHAPTER VI1 THE SPIRIT OF THE CHILD insufficient attention has been given to the child’s contribution to the subject. In the present day, however, we are concentrating on the study of the very early stages of child life, and are recognizing the great importance of that hitherto comparatively neglected, but exceptionally fertile, field of investigation, the pre-school period. Distinguished observers have, as is well known, _ rendered great service by giving to the world the valuable results of their detailed studies of the first year of the life of the child. If only equally careful investigations had been carried on in the intervening years, between the first and the entry of the child into the infants’ school, where we have an abundance of available material for research, it would have thrown a flood of light on the subject of the development in the early stages of a sense of humour and the appreciation of situations at which children laugh, and, moreover, of their intimate connexion with the play impulse. The third year of the child’s life is, in this connexion, of supreme importance. For the first time he recognizes himself as being something clearly marked off from other elements in his environment. If one could establish the necessary personal relationship with an intelligent group of three-year-olds so that they would talk freely on simple domestic matters, in their outlook on life, what an interest- ing and valuable experience it would be. The insight into the spirit of the child before the claims of habit and conven- tion had warped it, would be of enormous value and interest. 67 I the theories of laughter, as we have already seen, cc 68 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER The first impressions of the meaning of words, the nature _of the material which was regarded by them as funny and ' caused laughter, the sights and sounds which amused them, | and those which resulted in any indication of fear, would . give a valuable starting point for acquiring a knowledge of , the real child. The change of attitude of the four-year-old, and the apparent reasons for the change, would give some indica- tion of the moulding character of the environment and possibly suggest improved means of preserving, unfettered, the most precious elements in the spirit of the child. The observation of further changes during the five-year-old stage would complete the cycle of the pre-school period. The knowledge obtained would be an important asset for the teacher in dealing with the entrant to the infants’ department of the school, and secure a certain continuity of treatment which is so essential in the development of the child. It would, moreover, do something to stem the torrent of waste of time involved in the breaking-up of undesirable habits in the early days of school life. The detailed study of very young children is difficult, but where experiments have been carried on, under more or less ideal conditions, with appropriate groups of children at different pre-school ages, by skilled observers, the results have exceeded the most sanguine expectations. The experiments have been so conclusive that they will encourage the enthusiastic lovers of children to persevere in their reasonable demands for better conditions for the normal development of the young child. In dealing with the spirit of the child, the above digres- sion appeared to be necessary, in order to show that there is frequently something lacking in the early stages of his development during the very important period of active habit-formation, when skilled attention and a suitable environment are full of possibilities for the future. They are essential for securing that freedom of outlook, and the - THE SPIRIT OF THE CHILD 69 encouragement of that native ability for thinking for him- self, which are of the greatest importance, and should be the natural heritage, and the priceless possession, of every child. Reared in an atmosphere of authoritative ‘ don’ts ’ the child has little chance of developing a wholesome sense of humour, and the springs of laughter are injuriously affected at their source. Habits are obviously necessary for the general well-being of the child. Without them there would be a perpetual state of unrest, and the establishment of a definite per- sonality would be impossible. A good character naturally results from a nexus of desirable habits. Conduct along a particular line in the child’s attitude to life, and the natural response to given situations, are summarized in the habit. Once firmly fixed, the child having reacted again and again to a particular stimulus in the same way, the habit, in a sense, takes over the natural response, and his mind is at rest. He can now think about other things. To-morrow, if the same situation arises, he will, in all probability, behave as he behaves to-day. Some fresh experience comes across the child’s path, and he has to make a decision with regard to it. It is likely to recur. He may take advice and follow the suggested method of response, or he may think the matter out for himself and decide upon a line of action. If it succeeds he will probably react in the same way when he has to deal with a similar experience again, and eventually he forms a habit, and, again, his mind is at rest. Now, the habit is an excellent servant but a very bad master. There is a serious danger of becoming ‘ a creature of habits’. Here comes in the spirit of the child, with its freshness of outlook and its dominance of the habit-formation. The °creature of habits ’ has practically lost the spirit of the child, he has no growing points, they are buried in habits, and he has lost the power of forming any judgments which are alien to those habits. 70 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER In old age the definite routine, any departure from which is resented, clearly marks the devotion to habit and the departure of the spirit of the child. Take an extreme case : the really old man who rises at a fixed hour, is punctual at meals, has strong views with regard to diet, goes for his morning walk at the usual hour, visits his club, is annoyed if some stranger has taken his favourite chair with the | accustomed outlook, reads the paper which suits his | political views, has strong opinions with regard to any modern movements, has a great contempt for new fashions —especially feminine—in dress, compares the world of to-day with that of his youth, to the great advantage of the latter, takes his afternoon nap, and so on, day after day, with little variation. This type is to be met with at every club. The man is encrusted in habit and has become an automaton. The outlook of the child has practically no message for him. The age of a man is generally estimated on a chrono- logical basis, but his real age depends on the extent to which he has retained the spirit of the child. A man is old when he ceases, mentally, to have growing points, and can make no effective response to a new idea. Physiologists, on the other hand, tell us that old age has come when we cease to respond satisfactorily to thyroid extract. Some men are old at forty, others are delightfully young at eighty years of age ; the chronological basis of age is most unsatis- factory. | Freud's suggestion of the connexion of witticisms with the submission of the basic material to childish methods of dealing with situations is of great interest. Unquestionably, the appreciation of humorous material is largely concerned with the extent to which we have retained the spirit of the child. It is only in comparatively rare cases, however, that the spirit of the child has become entirely submerged. On special occasions it is revived if only for a brief period. It appears that pleasure derived from humorous appeal may THE SPIRIT OF THE CHILD 71 be said to vary directly with the extent to which the spirit of the child has been retained. Joy in laughter is related so intimately with the play impulse that we generally find elderly people—though chronology, as we have seen, has little to do with it—who laugh heartily at appropriate times, have great sympathy with other childish pleasures. They are immensely popular with children, who have no difficulty in distinguishing between genuine mirthfulness and an assumed hilarity. A child has an almost uncannily sound judgment of what he terms ‘ a really good sort ’, as distinguished from the person who simply wishes to make himself agreeable. Bergson, who gives a somewhat exaggerated estimate of absent-mindedness as a cause of laughter, attributes this failing to a want of mental alertness resulting from a mechanical type of mind, which, from our present point of view, would result from an excessive development of the habit-formation, bringing it into line with automatism. The absence of the spirit of the child, with its untrammelled outlook, would naturally give the necessary explanation of this attitude of mind. Happy joyous children are very rarely absent-minded, but they laugh heartily at those who are. Bergson, as we have seen, suggests that one of the functions of laughter is to prevent absent-mindedness because people do not like being laughed at. An important reason for pleading for greater attention being given to the study of the pre-school child in the interest, incidentally, of the development of humour and the springs of laughter, is that the two to five stage of child life is exceptionally rich in material of great value for the purpose. This is, par excellence, the period of naiveté, in connexion with which it is of paramount importance that the position of the spirit of the child should be safeguarded. It cannot be too often emphasized that a congenial atmosphere, and full opportunities being given for freedom of expression, are absolute necessities 72 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER for its natural development. Any repressive action at the beginning, through thoughtlessness or want of sufficient care, may permanently injure the spirit of the child. In the joyous environment of a good infants’ school with skilled and sympathetic teachers it is comparatively safe. In the early-Victorian home the spirit of the child was, in the first few years, in a much safer environment than in the modern home. The position of the mother was, as regards her relation to her lord and master, a somewhat subordinate, and, judged by modern standards, an unenvi- able one. Domesticity was her prime concern, she con- centrated on her children and the affairs of the home and apparently was a very happy woman. She knew her children far better than the modern mother does, and they had a very good time. She was not concerned to any serious extent with outside activities, and she was blissfully ignorant of politics. Then came the higher education, and, what is generally known as, the emancipa- tion of women. Her interests were then divided between external and internal activities, and the children were left much more to governesses and servants, and saw far less of their mother. In the later stages, the spirit of the child had probably some advantage from development under more highly skilled guidance, but there can be no doubt that, in the great period of the two to five years, the partial loss of the mother’s undivided attention was a serious matter. There is considerable evidence, however, that a strong back current has set in, and that the charms of domesticity are again making their appeal. With a more highly educated mother, returning of her own free will to the satisfying delights of the nursery, there may be a much more appro- priate atmosphere in the future for the spirit of the child. Any want of harmony in the home has an evil influence on the natural development of the child, the far reaching effect of which it would be impossible to over-estimate. THE SPIRIT OF THE CHILD 73 ~ The state of preservation of the spirit of the child largely determines, as we have seen, the nature of the response to humorous situations. However, even where the conditions have been most favourable for its retention, there must naturally be some diminution in its activities with advancing years. Various elements in it are gradually incorporated in habits which, though they may be of an excellent type, leave the spirit of the child somewhat impoverished. It is the joyous freedom, the fresh outlook, the power of dealing with a new situation, unencumbered with solutions suggested by neighbouring habits, which give the spirit of the child its unique value, and its daring in casting aside the conventions which would, in the normal order of things, control the behaviour. The attitude towards Christmas is the best possible test for the existence of the spirit of the child. More than that, we have in the attitude the power of gauging the relative amount of it retained in different cases. In other words, the Christmas test is quantitative as well as qualitative. Christmas, being by common consent, the children’s festival, means the temporary reversal of the normal routine of the home. The children’s wishes, to a very large extent, directly or indirectly dominate the situation. In the struggle for supremacy in the nursery King Baby must eventually be deposed to have to conform to ordinary discipline. Here, at Christmas time, however, the child returns to his kingdom, and, to a considerable extent, the order of discipline is relaxed, and the spirit of the child finds its purest expression. The extent to which this atmosphere makes its appeal to each of us will vary with the amount of the spirit of the child which we have retained. The order of the Christmas festivities has of recent years undergone some change. The visit of Santa Claus on Christmas Eve, with the mysterious filling of the children’s socks and stockings, is declining in popularity. The important innovation of surprise-packets with substantial 74 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER presents on the breakfast table on Christmas morning, to be followed at night by the joys of Father Christmas and the Christmas tree, replace other pleasures. By those adults, who have not an all-absorbing love of children, this diversion from its normal function of an important meal- time is resented. Many would gladly revert to the good old days of Santa Claus, when the riotous excitement of receiving presents had largely abated before the period was reached for laying a substantial foundation for the strenuous activities of Christmas day. An ineffective breakfast to a man of regular dietary habits might lead to an evaporation of the Christmas spirit long before the Christmas tree stage is reached. This is not all ; Christmas fare is notoriously indigestible, and indigestion is a front-rank cause of night phantasies in the réle of nightmare or vivid dreams. In spite of all this, however, the spirit of the child element triumphs. Our hygienic views with regard to meals are thrown on one side, and we delight in indigestible food, with the inevitable gastronomic disturbances which will naturally result from such dietetic indiscretion. It is sometimes urged that our behaviour at Christmas is simply a concession to family life. On analysing our feelings, we find, however, that it is not so. There is, in our heart of hearts, a feeling of joy that we have retained the power of entering once more into the happy, care-free kingdom of the child. The concession-to-family-life theory can be shown in other ways to have no substantial basis in connexion with the emergence at Christmas time of the spirit of the child. In an interesting experience of Christmas at sea in a large liner, the evidence appeared conclusive that it was the spirit of the child finding expression that made the Christ- mas festivity a conspicuous success. Various nationalities were represented, the family element was, of course, lacking, and the influence of a religious factor with such diverse peoples could have little effect. It was not merely THE SPIRIT OF THE CHILD 75 a festival spirit. There was a clearly-marked temporary lull in that sharp division into groups which are to be found on board ship. We felt that, for the time being, we were happy wanderers in the fairyland of childhood and we were glad to be there, CHAPTER VIII THE BEGINNINGS OF LAUGHTER connexion of the smile with the laugh. In the reports of the first year of child life by trustworthy observers we find, as we should expect, considerable differences in the time of the first appearance of significant features in the different stages of the course of development. The placid look of contentment after feeding may easily be mistaken for a smile by the fond mother, but the genuine smile after some action by the observer, which is repeated on the same, or a similar incident, is trustworthy evidence. Preyer’s child is said to have smiled on the tenth day, and to have laughed between the sixth and ninth week, but the general verdict is that the date of the first smile is at about the end of the third week, and that of the first laugh at about the end of the third month. Darwin’s two children did not smile before they were six weeks old, but the younger child laughed twenty days after the first smile, which is an abnormally short interval ~ between the smile and the laugh. Miss Milicent Shinn, who made a very valuable and trustworthy investigation of the development of her niece, Ruth, during the first year of the child’s life, gives a very interesting account of it in her fascinating book, The Biography of a Baby, which every mother should read. Miss Shinn describes the first appearance of the smile as follows: ‘The first smile that I could conscientiously record occurred the day before the baby was a month old, and it was provoked by the touch of a finger on her lips; and a day or two later she smiled repeatedly at touches on her lip.” ~ 76 M = references have already been made to the THE BEGINNINGS OF LAUGHTER 4d Between the advent of the smile and the laugh, Ruth frequently made sounds accompanying her smiles. These were described as gurgling, cooing, crowing and chuckling ; then, in the fourth month, came the definite laugh. ‘ From early in the month she acquired an ecstatic little chuckle, and once or twice even broke into a genuine laugh when she was played with a little more boisterously than usual.’ She laughed at a grimace in the fifth month which was definitely a laugh of amusement at the incongruous. It is interesting to notice, in this connexion, that one of Sully’s boys laughed between the fourth and the fifth month at some comic antics of his sister, and at the age of six months the sense of anything incongruous was a fertile source of laughter. The laughter of young children may be clearly shown to be of two kinds, the laughter of pleasure in the general well-being of the child, especially at the completion of any experiment successfully performed; and the laughter of amusement at comical incidents in which the incongruous plays a very important part. Funny sights, strange sounds, and unfamiliar combinations of domestic arrange- ments, all enter into the field of laughter-provoking incidents. The bath-room is the natural home of much ‘hilarity. If properly conducted, the bath is an excep- tionally pleasing experience, the joy of being without hindering clothes, the splashing in warm water, and the conditions being so different from those of daily routine, give unbounded satisfaction and pleasure to the healthy baby. From very early days sudden disappearance—now seen, now gone—makes a very strong appeal. It is summarized in the Bo-péep game, which is immensely popular and retains its popularity for a long time. If the child is in a fretting condition, a game of Bo-peep will frequently act as a restorative, and smiles and laughter again enter the baby’s kingdom. Many of the games which come after a 78 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER babyhood is passed are based upon the principles of this earlier delight. The Jack-in-the-box with its element of surprise, sometimes at the beginning with a slight sense of fear—so soon dissipated—is the starting point for the long series of mirth-provoking games based on surprise, followed by a relief from restraint. It is not confined to games ; many of the most delightful stories and humorous situations may be traced back to this foundation. Bo-peep and Jack-in-the-box and their successors are, in fact, sometimes regarded as the main sources of children’s laughter. In treating of the beginnings of laughter a further reference to tickling is necessary. Special attention is drawn by Darwin to the necessity, in successful tickling, of the particular areas touched being those which are generally guarded against attack. The sensation does not arise if the point to be touched is previously indicated by the tickler ; the element of surprise must be present. A child cannot tickle himself because he knows which point in the ticklish area will be affected. Tickling is not purely a physiological matter, there is an emotional factor. The tickled child, if laughter is to result, must be on friendly terms with the tickler and, moreover, he must be in a playful attitude. Tickling by a stranger would be resented, and the response would be anger or tears, not laughter. The theory that tickling is valuable in preparation for skill in defence from attack does not concern us here. In most of the records of detailed observation on the first year of child life, it is noticed that, at certain periods, the childish play becomes unduly vigorous, and an element of rowdiness makes its appearance, accompanied by loud, and sometimes harsh, laughter. In the case of Miss Shinn’s niece it was first in evidence in the early weeks of the seventh month, lasted for a short time, and then there was a return to normal behaviour. The desire for boisterous fun at certain intervals does not appear to be entirely associated with an unusually robust state of THE BEGINNINGS OF LAUGHTER 79 health. It seems to occur fitfully, with no apparent reason, as a phase in ordinary development. In the boisterous fun the tickling sensation, with the continual surprises involved, undoubtedly enters in. The variations which are observed to occur in the sensitiveness of the skin in particular areas, from time to time, may have something to do with the fleeting interest in mimic fights in the life of the baby. A satisfactory animal, dog or cat, makes an abnormal appeal to the ordinary baby, and is the cause of much laughter. Here and there we meet with cases of a fear of the domestic animal, which might probably be traced to a rough response to the attentions of a persistent baby. The extraordinary pleasure a developing child takes in pulling hair, to whomsoever it belongs, must be a severe trial to the temper of the nursery pet. Quite apart from the hair-pulling interest, the baby derives great pleasure from watching animals. In many respects the dog is far more interesting than the nurse. There is a greater variety in its movements, and the baby never knows what a dog will do next. It is just a delightful series of sur- prises ; whereas, in the nurse’s movements, there is very little variety, and the intelligent baby soon gets a fairly good idea of what she will do next. There is some- thing wrong with the nursery from which animals are banished. In connexion with the first appearance of imitation in the baby’s development, and any possible influence in regard to the act of laughing, Miss Shinn’s record is of considerable value. ‘I have already mentioned certain doubtful imitations in the fourth month and a clearer one in the fifth. Now, the baby began to imitate unmistak- ably. Her uncle had a fashion of slapping his hand down on the table by way of a salutation to her, and one day (when she had passed a week of her sixth month) she slapped down her little hand in return. The next day as 80 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER soon as her uncle came in, she began to slap her hand down watching him, delighted to repeat the movements back and forth, as long as he would keep it up’. This, which is in keeping with the results of other observers, disposes of the suggestion that laughter depends upon imitation. Imitation comes on the scene after the laughing act is firmly established. It would appear that laughter comes naturally when the neuro-muscular mechanism is sufficiently developed for its production. Prior to this, as we have seen, when the baby is in exceptionally good spirits smiles are accompanied by sounds which, at the appointed time, reached the final stage of laughter. The laugh of amusement is, undoubtedly, stimulated by incongruous sights and sounds during baby- hood. In learning to walk the child appears to delight in mishaps, such as sitting down with a thud during an interval of standing, as it gives rise to uproarious merri- ment. It is evidently regarded by the child as being very comical. An amusing incident is related by Miss Shinn of her niece, in learning to talk. She had acquired a curious sound to indicate ‘all gone’, which she frequently used for any act of disappearance. On putting her hand on her father’s bald head she used the same expression. But to attach any humorous intent to this, would be to suggest too high a standard of intelligence for a baby just reaching the end of her first year. The laughter of children in the pre-school period, as far as funny sights are concerned, follows the line of those indicated in the babyhood period. They laugh at the funny antics of the domestic animals, and anything of an incongruous character in the events of the home. Any form of dressing up in weird costumes is a delight to them, and they laugh heartily at abrupt changes in the daily routine. The minor misfortunes of others, provided that their intimate friends are not implicated, are the cause of much merriment. Boisterous fun plays an THE BEGINNINGS OF LAUGHTER 81 important part in everyday life, and it is accompanied by shrieks of laughter. Quick action and constant change are elements of popular appeal. If taken to the Zoo the monkey-house is a glorious experience for them, and a ride on the elephant is long remembered. With very young children, however, the noise in the parrot-house may have a somewhat terrifying effect. The third year of the child’s life is, as we have seen, of very special interest and a period of great development. The following short extracts from the record of a competent observer on his little boy at this age, shows the variety of interests which have already made their appearance : (a) ‘I asked him one morning when he awoke if he had a kiss for me, and he replied, “ No. I haven’t got the new ones in yet.” I then said, “ Haven't you any left over from last night ? ”’ and he replied, “ Only one or two and they are rather dirty and have got holes in them. But I can give you one of these if you like ”.’ (b) ‘ His mother said when she was putting him to bed, “If I had a little daughter what would she be to you?” He replied promptly, “ She’d be no use to ne”) (c) ‘ Told that he was going to Jersey in the summer, he asked immediately, “ Do the people all wear jerseys there? ”’ (d) * When I told him that we were going to Kew Gardens, he asked, “ Do they make cucumbers there ? ”’’ The boy always delighted in playing with words for which he had considerable aptitude. At the age of five he did not show the same delight in playing with words, but occasionally he made excellent puns. He became interested in other things, and some- times moralized, as the following extracts from the record show : (a) ‘ He is always ready to say “ I am sorry ” if he does wrong. But to-day, he was very slightly disobedient, and 6 ro —— 82 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER when I said, “ Won't you say you are sorry ? ” he promptly quoted “ The walrus and the carpenter ”, by replying, “ But answer came there none ”’ (6) “ A friend dining with us saw him in bed with a plush cat, three bears and a golliwog. ‘ Oh,” she said, ““ does mummy take a lot of things to bed with her ? ”’ to which he replied, *“ No, only daddy ".’ (c) ‘ Built a house with toy bricks. I commented upon its nine chimneys. “ Yes,” he said, “ and do you know, daddy, there's a special invention of my own in that house. When the door of a room is opened, it strikes a flint and. lights the fire (@) ‘ Last night the boy was unhappy because we were going out to dine. To his mother he said, ““ I don’t think life is worth living ; sometimes I wish I had never been born.” Later, I asked him if he really wished he had never been born, and he replied, *“ Sometimes I do, some- times I don’t”. (¢) ‘He showed a good deal of temper to-day, but recovered a little later. When asked by his mother why he had been so bad-tempered, he replied, “ Ah! that’s the question. I find great difficulty in answering it ”’.’ The extracts are given in chronological order. The last refers to an incident which occurred just before his sixth birthday. The boy, to whom the record refers, was an only child, in a home of considerable culture. He spent much of his time in the presence of his parents, who were devoted to him. Apparently he did not care much for the company of other children, and preferred that of grown-ups. During the pre-school period the nature of the playing with words, which is a great source of laughter from 2 to 5 years of age, must of necessity be affected, to a remarkable degree, by the type of environment. A similar record of a child reared with other children in a typical lower middle-class home would be of great interest and value. A comparison THE BEGINNINGS OF LAUGHTER 83 of the records, as far as they concern the beginnings of laughter, would, moreover, be most illuminating. Reliable stories about very young children are valuable as showing the rapid change of outlook from year to year. The Jenny Lind Book of Children’s Sayings, by Miss Helen C. Colman, is of considerable interest in this connexion. The sayings are arranged in chronological order, and give clear evidence of a developing sense of humour, starting with the two-year-old stage. To give a few examples: A child of two years makes a pun, knowing-the curious combination of words to be funny as shown by his ‘ roguish smile ’. At this age, a girl, seeing a coating of ice on the water-butt, suggested that it was ‘the fairies’ birthday cake ’, and another interested in looking at some tadpoles, and, hearing her mother say that later on the tails dropped off and their legs appeared, exclaimed, ‘ what did you do with my tail, mummy ? And weren't you ¢ncited when my legs began to grow ? ’ The anthropomorphic explanations of spiritual things by the child are of special interest. A girl of three years on going to church, and hearing that God was always there, asked what He had for dinner, and, not receiving a satis- factory reply, said cheerfully, ‘ Oh, I expect He'll have an egg for His tea.” A good example of the same point is the case quoted of an older child who, on the day that his grandmother died, jumped into bed without saying his prayers. Asked why he did this, he replied, ‘ Oh, well, I don’t think I ought to bother God to-night as He will be busy settling granny in’. Discussions on Bible narratives are particularly productive of interesting examples of the child’s attitude. Thus, a four-year-old boy in answer to a question as to why Abraham lived in a tent, said, ‘ Be- cause Abraham was a Boy Scout’. A girl of the same age, after a Bible talk about Rebecca, was asked by her mother why it was that Rebecca went to her wedding on a camel. She replied, ‘ because there must have been a 84 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER railway strike’. The imperfect understanding of words is responsible for the child’s description of the occupants of the fiery furnace as Shadrach, Meshach, and a bad negro. The fairy story has some effect on the beginnings of laughter. In the ordinary home, when speech is fairly well understood, and an effort is made to interest the child, pride of place is given to fairy stories. The picture books for the children are largely concerned with illustrations of these stories. The richness of the fairy-story literature gives it a great advantage over the domestic story. In fact, the children on learning to read, have a greater and more varied series of children’s books from the great writers of different nationalities, dealing with the fairy element, than they will have at any subsequent period. The classical fairy story, well translated, and admirably illustrated, reaches a far higher standard than the other children’s books, and, moreover, those which compete with it, such as Alice in Wonderland, have a distinct fairy-element basis. The young child in a fairly good home knows all about the fairy stories long before he can read them, and one of the greatest incentives to the child to learn to read is to be able to enjoy them for himself. The quickening of the imagination, and variety of interests aroused, give a whole- some stimulus to the sense of humour and its expression in laughter. The Three Bears, Jack and the Beanstalk, Ali Baba, Cinderella, and Dick Whittington and his Cat are full of delightfully humorous situations. Following closely upon the heels of the fairy stories, come the children’s animal stories, which are immensely popular. They are linked up with the fairy stories, for the animals are generally endowed with characteristic human qualities. The literary and artistic standard of the animal story has advanced considerably of recent years, and its beneficent influence on the beginnings of laughter is beyond question. : The influence of fairy and animal stories has had, as we THE BEGINNINGS OF LAUGHTER 85 shall see later on, a marked effect on the child’s attitude to animals. The extension of the interest created by the exaggerated opinion of the intelligence of the domestic pet, gives the dog or the cat a better claim to the close personal friendship of the child. Even in the stories of children of g or 10 years of age, we find the fairy element in their accounts of animal behaviour. The naiveté of the child, with his delight in playing with words, reaches its highest point during the pre-school period. It abounds in humorous material, due to the misunderstanding of words as a result of the young child talking and singing before he can apply the visual test to the words employed. When the child sings lustily, * When shepherds washed their frocks by night * he knows nothing about watching flocks, and, after all, there is little difference in sound, and the familiar replaces the unfamiliar. In many of the hymns sung by children, the real words and their meaning come as a revelation when the written form is presented. In the meantime, the sounds and the rhythm are all that appeal to the child mind. The following naive story is not without interest, as showing the child’s attitude to moral values: ‘A child of 3% years told her mother that she had not broken some partridges eggs, of which she was suspected. But at prayer time the following was overheard: ‘‘ Please, God, mend those eggs what I kicked, and tell the muvver bird to come back again ’. Thereupon, the mother said reproachfully, “ You did do it then ”, to which the child retorted, “I was talking to God and you was rude to listen. I will say my prayers in my stomach to-morrow *’.’ In a chapter on ‘ The Age of Naiveté’, in The Child's Attitude to Life, I have dealt in some detail with the subject. The following extract indicates the nature of the child’s difficulty in appreciating the meaning of words : ‘ The slow process of discovering the real meaning of words, which the children use quite fearlessly, naturally 86 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER results in a rich harvest of amusing mistakes. To follow the meaning attached to words through the various stages, ufitil they reach the normal content which the outer world recognizes as satisfactory, would be a very laborious task. Moreover, it would probably be of little value, as the process of reaching the ultimate goal may be short-circuited by the intelligent child, and would depend so largely on the opportunities of hearing a particular word used in different connexions. There is the greatest possible variety even in adult life in the knowledge of the content of many words in common use. The suggestion of a small child that the reason why our first parents were forbidden to taste the fruit of a particular tree was, ““ Because they were cooking apples ”’, was quite natural, and probably repre- sented the result of a childish experience. Little children are much interested in the story of the Garden of Eden, and the child’s question, as to the kind of carriage in which Adam and Eve were driven out of the Garden, might well indicate the child’s normal acquaintance with the word “driven ”’. On hearing for the first time the banns of marriage published in church with the final words, * This is for the third time of asking ”’, it was not unreasonable that a little girl should say, ‘“ Mother, dear, how did the clergyman know how many times the man had asked her?’ Children, at the age of naiveté, frequently use very picturesque language in the description of normal occur- rences ; for example, a child, describing a scene between a dog and cat, concluded by saying, “ And then the cat went up to the dog and she sneezed right in his face’. Another child in describing a visit to the Zoo said, * When we threw some biscuits to the elephant, he picked them up with his vacuum-cleaner.”’ ’ A little boy went fishing for the first time but did not catch anything, and remarked sorrowfully to his brother, ‘I don’t think my worm is trying ’. THE BEGINNINGS OF LAUGHTER 87, In some cases, as we have seen, the passage from naiveté to humour is materially influenced by the effect produced upon the child, as a result of the amusement caused by his words, which were uttered with no humorous intent. CHAPTER IX WHAT YOUNG CHILDREN LAUGH AT (FIVE TO TWELVE YEARS OF AGE) HE springs of laughter at the pre-school stage are largely funny sights and sounds, boisterous fun, and an elementary playing with words; and, in a well ordered home, contact has been made by fairy stories and animal stories with a rich store of interesting literature. Picture books are of absorbing interest, and the child is finding his way rapidly, by his own efforts at reading, to become less dependent on others for the explanation of his pictures. Verbal humour is thus gradually entering into competition with visual humour as a source of childish pleasure, and playing with words is reaching a higher standard. It is interesting to observe at what an early age words are treated like new toys in the nursery, and unusual combinations of them enlarge the field for laughter. Such strings of words as ‘ A noisy noise annoys an oyster most’ are very popular with young children. Many children soon acquire an aptitude for punning, in which similarity of sound is associated with contrast of meaning, and this gives great scope for a variety of word games. In order to find out what children laugh at at different ages an investigation was made, in connexion with which some thousands of children wrote accounts of the funniest stories they had ever read or heard, the jokes which had made them laugh the most, and the sights they had seen which they considered to be the most comical. In order to get the impressions of children in the infants’ school, who could not give the information in writing, they were asked 88 WHAT YOUNG CHILDREN LAUGH AT 89 questions and their answers recorded. In each case the age and sex of the child were given. In this way it was possible to obtain a survey of the kind of material which provokes most laughter through a range of approximately thirteen years from verbal and visual sources. In the present chapter we shall deal with the laughter of children from 5 to 12 years of age and in the following chapter with that of boys and girls from 12 to 18 years of age. Verbal humour appears to run along the lines of a logical sense of incongruity and of reasoning ability ; whereas, visual humour appears to depend rather on emotional development. It is comparatively easy to trace the continuity and development of the former, which is closely associated throughout with the play impulse, but with the latter it is very difficult to trace its development along equally definite lines. Humour appears to correlate very highly with intelligence. In the stories of a large group of American children the intelligence quotients were given, and the correlation appeared to be very marked. Other evidence from English children clearly points in the same direction. With a more careful and scientific investigation many interesting problems might be attacked. The great differences at certain points of the sense of humour in boys and girls is of much interest, and the influence of environ- ment seems to have a marked effect on humour. The compensatory value of humour in the less fortunate sections of the population is also a matter of considerable importance and interest. In fact, the whole subject bristles with fascinating problems for research, which are beyond the scope of the present inquiry as to what children laugh at at different ages. Two points, however, emerge very clearly: At the period of rapid growth, from about 11} years to the end of the thirteenth year, there is a great falling off in the 90 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER sense of verbal humour of both boys and girls, but this is gradually regained when the curve of growth flattens out again. The nature of the sights which children laugh at —visual humour—appears to be affected much less, apart from an increase in the stories of the misfortunes-of-others variety, but in describing them the extravagant language, characteristic of this period, is nearly always used. The second point which emerges is that, in analysing the stories, it is found that certain types, as we shall see, seem to have a more or less definite age limit, above which they rarely occur. The funny sights, on the other hand, representing the same type of humorous situation, still make their appeal, and are frequently quoted as those which caused most laughter. In other words, the childish funny sight retains its popularity longer than the funny stories dealing with similar material. In naive stories, especially those told by children beyond " the normal age of naiveté, there appears to be an uncon- scious appreciation of the humorous content, which apparently is unrecognized by the child. This may explain the reason for the natural transference from mnaiveté to humour which has already been noticed. In the social intercourse of children, the production of laughter appears to act as a sort of safety valve, and to remove sources of irritation, and thus restores the normal joyousness of child- hood. The ease with which tears can be turned to laughter in the nursery is well known, and with a group of quarrelling schoolchildren a well-timed joke may produce a similar change of attitude. Following the delight of playing with words during the pre-school period of the child’s life, there comes a clearly- marked stage in which the riddle plays a part among the springs of laughter. The girls throughout are more interested in the riddles than the boys, and, with them, the popularity of this form of amusement starts a year earlier. From 7 to 10 years of age with the girls, and 8 to 10 with WHAT YOUNG CHILDREN LAUGH AT 91 the boys, the riddle is referred to frequently in the choice of funny stories, and then comes a sudden decline in favour at II years; after which, with the exception of a short period of revival about five years later, the riddle is rarely mentioned in the records. Moreover, when the riddle reappears it is of a different type, and is based on scholastic material generally associated with geographical names or with literary sources. There is occasionally some compe- tition between boys and girls in the making of riddles, during this passing phase, and in this the girls prcve to be the more successful. With young children the extraordinary appeal of the fairy story naturally affects their selection of the particular type which causes them most amusement. At 7 years of age twelve per cent of the boys and twenty-five per cent of the girls quote stories of the fairy variety. At the age of 8 years the percentages are twenty-three and twenty-five respectively, at g they are twenty-two and thirty-one, and at 10 they are seventeen and twenty-three. Then there is a falling off to eight and nineteen per cent respectively at II years of age. After that period fairy stories are rarely selected by the boys, though at 12 years fourteen per cent of the girls remain faithful to stories of this nature. It will be seen from these figures that the girls are always ahead of the boys in their preference for fairy stories. In the domestic stories, selected during the range of 7 to I2 years, in the number of which the boys are somewhat ahead of the girls, there is frequently a considerable fairy element in the doings recorded of the animals associated with the home and also of those at the Zoo. It takes the form, as we have seen, of attributing abnormal qualities of intelligence and humour to animals which are generally assumed to be confined to human beings. In the descrip- tions of funny sights during this period a very large proportion centre around the remarkable behaviour of animals, and there is a much closer approximation of 92 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER visual to verbal sources of laughter than is to be found in other departments of mirth-provoking situations to be described later on. As examples of great intelligence among animals the following records of the children may be quoted : (a) ‘ The hedgehog lives in the country. It is a clever little thing. Whenever a cow lies down to have a sleep the little hedgehog crawls up beside him. The cow is very heavy, and he presses the ground down. When the little worms feel the cow on top of them they come out of their holes to see what it is. The hedgehog knows what is going to happen, so he looks out for all the worms that he can possibly find. When he has had enough to eat he crawls away. Foxes are very fond of hedgehogs, and they would eat them if they could, but they cannot reach them.’ (6) ‘ One day when I was at the Zoo, and it was about lunchtime, all the people were sitting on seats which were right down the pathway. Along this path all the elephants came, carrying little children for a ride. One lady, who had a great many children, had taken some of their lunch out of her attaché-case, and put the rest under the seat again. Just then an elephant came along, and he saw the attaché-case, and went nearer to get it. All the while the children were running away and screaming. When the elephant saw they had gone, he picked up the attaché-case and the lunch which was inside, and then he walked on again. After he had been round a great many times, he dropped the attaché-case under the right seat, but there was not any lunch inside it.’ (¢) ‘I think the funniest sight I ever saw was a goat with three legs. It had two hind-legs and one in the centre at the front. It had two kids which each had four legs. The way it hopped about in the field was very queer and it played with its kids in a most ludicrous manner. It never seemed to lament its loss, and it always seemed in good health. People very often are heard to say, “as silly as a WHAT YOUNG CHILDREN LAUGH AT 93 goat ”’, but I disagree with them, in this case at any rate. One night we had burglars. The farmer had a collection of old plate, which had descended from father to son and had been in the family for years. To obtain an entrance ‘to the farmhouse, you had to come through the paddock where the goat was kept at night. The burglars, therefore, had to pass by the goat, there being no hedges round the paddock. It was a dark night so they hoped to pass unseen or heard. But the corner of the paddock was piled with twigs and one of the burglars trod on them. Snap! The goat was awake in an instant and bleated till the dogs heard it. They rushed in and pinned the burglars to the ground, while the farmer ran out with his men, and handed them over to the police. The goat, *“ Three-legs ”’ they called her, was petted all the next day, not by us children only, but even the farmer gave her some extra food to eat.’ (d) ‘ Our neighbours have a cat called Fluffy. One day she was in our house and she jumped from the arm-chair on to the top of daddy’s desk knocking a vase down ; then opening the window with one paw she pushed it up with the other and went after the birds.’ (¢) ‘I saw- a spider's web in our kitchen. I did put upon it a fly and the spider came running out and wrapped him up and ate him for his tea. It made me laugh.’ (f) ‘I saw a cat teaching another how to open a door by standing on her hind legs and pressing down the latch with her paws and then pushing it. I had to stuff my handker- chief into my mouth to suppress my laughter. It was too funny for words.’ Many stories are told by the children of the remarkable intelligence of monkeys, and the wonderful remarks of parrots overheard at the Zoo. Occasionally, stories are told showing that animals give evidence of possessing fine moral qualities as, for example : ~ (a) ‘Ihave a cat who has a kitten. One day my mother 94 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER gave the kitten some fish. I was very surprised to see the kitten walk away, and start mewing as if she was calling somebody. I showed her the fish in vain, she would not eat it. Just then the mother cat came in and the kitten at once took her to the fish, and they ate it together. Then we understood ; the kitten would not eat the fish until her | mother had some. I think the kitten was very unselfish, | but it was very funny that she would not eat the fish without her mother.’ | (6) “ Our cat is very fond of our rabbit and likes to play | with him in the garden. One day she took a cabbage leaf | to him and kissed him. She will sit on his hutch every | morning until he is let out to play with her.’ (c) “ A goose fought a cock because he drove some little | children out of a poultry yard and he gave him a really | good beating. It taught the cock a lesson and now he never drives children out of the yard.’ (d) “ A little dog bit a small boy’s nose, and he was so . sorry afterwards that he made a great fuss of him and >. played with him and put his paws round his neck and) kissed him.’ (¢) “ Our dog used to dislike our kitten but at last they: made it up, and one day when I went into the room I saw the dog asleep on the rug and his paw was curled round his is late enemy, the kitten.’ The affection of one animal for another is a frequent ir element in the girls’ stories but very rarely in those of thei: boys. Many of the children’s records also suggest that animals/s. possess a sense of humour. After a graphic account of a 1. hen successfully fighting a dog who had interfered with i. her chickens, the story concluded with, ‘and when the mother hen pecked the dog’s nose the chickens cacklec:i. with laughter ’. In describing an incident of a large dog, and a small fox-terrier who had befriended a small strange. kitten, and had given her a good washing, the story goein, se nD WHAT YOUNG CHILDREN LAUGH AT 95 on to relate, ‘ The kitten raised no objection, and then the dogs kissed her and they winked at each other.” In an amusing account of a fowl who shared the family life, and came into the breakfast-room each morning, and would lay an egg in a box under the table or iu someone’s lap, the record states: ‘The hen always smiled at us when she came into the room in the morning.’ A favourite story with the children was that of a chicken who asked her mother whether she might have a little brother, to which she replied, ‘What! With eggs at fourpence each, certainly not.” A child, having heard that a hyena in the Zoo laughed all day, but rarely had anything to eat or drink, said, Then what has he got to laugh about.’ The misfortunes of others as a cause of laughter are frequently referred to by young children, and form the basis of many funny stories. With children of 7 years of age, about twenty-five per cent of the boys’ stories, and sixteen per cent of the girls’ are of this nature. At 8 years of age there is a decrease to about eighteen and ten per cent respectively. At 9 and 10 years there is a further very considerable reduction, and beyond the age of 10 the proportion is negligible. At first, the boys are far ahead of the girls in their choice of this type of humorous situation, but beyond the age of 8 years the difference is less marked. With animal stories there is a certain similarity between the funny story or joke and the descrip- tion of the funny sight recorded, but with the misfortunes of others as the basis there is no such relation. The misfortunes-of-others story practically ceases after the age of 10 with both boys and girls. Not so with funny sights of the misfortunes-of-others type. As we have seen, they retain their popularity long after the age at which this kind of funny story has disappeared, and descriptions of them are in special favour during the period of rapid growth from 12 to 14 years of age. This parting of the ways of the appeal of verbal and 96 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER visual humour is of special interest. It has been pointed out that the humorous situation appropriate for laughter has varied from century to century, depending upon the social attitude of the period in question. McDougall also has, as we have seen, insisted on the important function of laughter, in connexion with the minor misfortunes of others, in the safeguarding of the principle of primitive passive sympathy. As a matter of fact, even among adults, a large proportion of our laughter is concerned with the misfortunes of others. We laugh at the mistakes of other people, especially those in official positions, and here the element of our own superiority finds gratification and increases the humorous appreciation of the situation. In many ways, the amusement caused by the misfortunes of others is an interesting resultant of different constituents. Laughter at the misfortunes of others is seen in its simplest form in the incidents which cause amusement to very young children. Thus, a little boy of 5 says: ‘I saw a fat man running for a bus. He jumped on but he couldn’t hold on and he fell off into the mud. Oh! I did laugh.” A girl of 6 gives as her funniest sight: ‘I saw a duck who got his head stuck in a jam-pot, and he walked about with the jam-pot stuck on his head.’ Whereas, a boy of 7 says, ‘ The funniest sight I have seen is when my mother fell out of the hammock ; but she didn’t hurt herself much.” The fact that she hurt herself, though not much, did not interfere with his enjoyment. In many of the springs of laughter the child in his account inserts a saving clause of this type, and the nature of it varies from age to age. Though very rarely in the boys’ stories there is not infrequently in the girls’ a moral indicated as for example ‘I saw a boy chasing a poor cat. As he was running he fell and burst the seat of his trousers, and so instead of hurting the cat he'll get hurt himself when he sees his mother.’ From 10 to 12 years of age the funny sights recorded of WHAT YOUNG CHILDREN LAUGH AT 97 the misfortunes of others deal mainly with domestic events of which the following are illustrations : (@) A girl of 10 spent her summer holiday at the seaside with her uncle and aunt, and they all went bathing. Her aunt dived from a breakwater where there was a lot of sea-weed in the water. ‘Presently I saw auntie’s pink bathing cap appear above the water and her feet kicking about, and uncle and I laughed. But there was still a funnier sight to be seen, for auntie came up covered with sea-weed, and she had two crabs biting her, and she cried out, “ Oh, my toe! oh, my leg!” She looked so comical that we laughed until the tears ran out of our eyes, and I still laugh whenever I think about it.’ (6) Two small boys were watching a big boy fishing from a tree over a lake. He slipped and fell into the water, and the record ends: ‘When he was safe on dry ground, dripping with water, we could no longer hold our suppressed mirth, and we held our hips and roared. What happened when he heard us laughing is another story.’ (c) A girl visited her uncle in the country : ‘ A man near the house we were in had a donkey, and one day he asked my uncle to fetch it into the stable. My uncle having coaxed the donkey near to him leapt on its back. To this heavy load the donkey objected. After galloping about for some time, he rushed wildly to a pond where there were lots of ducks and geese, and threw my uncle right into it. There was my uncle with the ducks and geese flying all round him. At last he picked himself up and fled, with the geese all running after him.’ (4d) ‘ One evening as I was walking along a main road I suddenly espied in the distance a man and woman. The man was very tall and frail and the woman very small and stout. They were both running to catch a bus which was on the point of starting. The man was taking long strides while his wife was puffing and blowing to keep up to him. They lost the bus and the man made angry remarks at his 7 98 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER wife for not running quicker, and the woman was standing beside him wiping the perspiration from her face which was very red. Then the two went stalking off to get the next one with very long faces.’ (e) ‘ A boy jumped on the back of a water cart to have a ride ; the driver saw him and turned on the water which drenched him. We laughed till our sides ached as the boy rolled down the hill.’ It is natural that, in the records of funny sights, the scenes in streets, trams, buses and trains should play a prominent part. With the exception of the seven-year-old period, far more sights of this type are described by the boys than by the girls, who spend more of their time in the home. This is especially the case at 11 and 12 years of age, where the proportions of the number of boys’ records to girls’ are as thirty-four to sixteen and forty-five to twenty- three respectively. In cases where the funny sights are associated with queer costumes and dressing up, however, the percentage of girls’ records is greatly in excess of that of the boys’ at all ages. The conductor on the top of a bus who asks, ‘ Which is the gent. with the two ladies inside ’ ; the stout gentleman who admonishes a boy who is sitting down while there are two ladies standing up in the bus, and is told by the boy that if he stood up both of them could sit down ; the two stout ladies who enter the tram, and one asks for two Elephants; and the little man wedged in between two fat women who asks if he may come up to breathe, all cause much amusement to the children. In the streets, the cat who walks on her hind legs and does the one-step, and takes her share in pushing a doll’s perambulator ; the man who in a windstorm runs after, and captures, the wrong umbrella ; the clever performing monkey and a variety of attractive shows greatly add to the enjoyment of the children. Among the records of street incidents described by the children are the following : WHAT YOUNG CHILDREN LAUGH AT 99 (a) © A fat woman was going for a walk with a frail man. They came to a puddle through which they had to walk, whereupon the woman said, *“ If you were half a man you would have carried me over that puddle,” and the frail man retorted, ““ If you had been half a woman I would have done so ”’.’ ; (6) ‘ A band was playing and a dog in the front row of the crowd jumped up and fetched two other dogs and they sat down together a few yards from the band. Then the three started howling and would not stop until the band moved on.’ (¢) A bishop was interested in some mud figures which had been made by a boy, and he asked him to explain whom they represented. After the boy had done so, he said, ‘ I see you haven't made a bishop’. ‘No!’ said the boy, ‘ I hadn’t enough mud for a bishop.’ We have seen that, at an early stage, the riddle is very popular ; but at about 10 years of age, after a period of comparative docility, the classroom becomes the scene of much laughter from a very different source. A critical attitude makes its appearance, and the children find pleasure in comical incidents concerned with the stupidity of their class-mates, and at passages of arms between teacher and child. Any failure of those in authority to come out successfully in a wordy dispute, or any ridiculous misunderstanding of instructions, are causes of much merriment. A boy is discovered eating sweets, and at the same time his feet are projecting beyond the natural confines of the desk space. The teacher in reproving him said : ‘ Take those sweets out of your mouth and put your feet in.” Her rebuke was the cause of much laughter. A similar reception was given to the response of the child who was asked in a Bible lesson to quote a Commandment containing four words, and said, ‘ Keep off the grass’. The explanation of a word by the teacher, and the request to the children to construct sentences containing the 100 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER word to see whether the meaning of the word is adequately grasped, frequently gives rise to interesting results. One of the most extravagant of those quoted in the records was in connexion with the word heroes ; for which the sentence ran: ‘I put a pin in my father’s chair, and directly after he sat down, %e rose’. Similar examples are given by children in different countries. The laughter caused in the classroom by the stupid replies of young children is largely due to the feeling of superiority of the others. A few examples may be given : (a) The teacher was talking about thankfulness and politeness, and told a story of the Indian Rajah who brought some lovely gems and beautiful silks for the king, and she asked the children what the king would say. To which a small boy replied, ‘ ‘Ow much for the lot.’ (b) The talk was about the wind ; and the teacher told how in coming to the school in the morning on the top of a bus, something came very softly and kissed her on the cheeks, and she asked, ‘ Now what was that, children ?’ Whereupon, a young child said, ‘ Oh, teacher, I think that must have been the conductor.’ (c) A teacher was telling the children about the functions of different parts of the body, and in the course of her remarks said, ‘ you have ears to hear, eyes to see, nose to smell, and feet to run.” On which, a young girl said, ‘I think I must have been made wrong teacher, for my nose runs and sometimes my feet smell.’ Mistakes made in examination are much enjoyed by older children who know the correct answers. It is suggested by some critics that schoolboy howlers are frequently manufactured by adults. In some cases this may be true, but the genuine answers by children rival in absurdity those from such questionable sources. It is interesting to compare reliable products with the actual questions set in the examination concerned. In an investigation of the scripts of about 5,000 children of the WHAT YOUNG CHILDREN LAUGH AT 101 ages of 11 and 12 years, some remarkable results were obtained. One of the questions set was, ‘ Write in a few words, what you know of any three of the following : Among the nine names of people and objects named in the list were included, Julius Cesar, Oliver Cromwell, Thomas Beckett, and Sir Walter Raleigh. Here are some of the answers : ‘ Julius Caesar betrayed our Lord for thirty pieces of silver,” and another child wrote : ‘ He conquered England, and reigned from 55 B.C. to A.D. 410.’ ‘Oliver Cromwell had a round head. He asked for more.’ ‘ Thomas Beckett was a great churchman. He gained favour of the king by giving useful advice at the king’s request to be divorced from his wife, Catherine of Aragon. When Queen Elizabeth came to the throne, he was tried for heresy and condemned to be burnt to death, because he said that he was a Roman Catholic, when he was really a Protestant. He put his right hand in the flame first, for that was the hand which had cost him his life.” Many of the boys said that Thomas Beckett was a great boxer. ‘ Sir Walter Raleigh laid down his cloak in the mud for Queen Victoria to step over.” Another child wrote : ‘ Sir Walter Raleigh discovered Virginia, and after that Queen Elizabeth was always called the Virginia Queen.’ Entertainments at cinemas, theatres, concerts, music- halls, and circuses, are immensely popular with young children and form the basis of many funny stories, and the descriptions of comical sights. There is, however, a great difference in the number of stories of this type recorded by the boys and girls. At the age of 7 and 8 years the boys’ stories are considerably in excess of the girls’, and in all cases up to 12 years of age the percentage for the boys is greater, though the difference is not so marked in the later as in the earlier years. It would appear that more boys than girls attend entertainments of this character. 102 ‘THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER Thus, as in connexion with street scenes, there is more boys’ laughter than girls’ outside the home. It is interest- ing, at a popular entertainment containing a variety of items, to notice the difference in the nature of the appeal of particular situations to the representatives of widely separated age groups in the audience. In accounts of laughter in the home at domestic incidents the girls’ records are much more numerous than the boys’. Such a story as the following is typical : ‘ One morning my mother’s bootlace was undone, and she tripped on it when she brought the porridge into the room, and she fell down and spilt it all. When dad went to help her up he made matters worse because he fell down too on to the porridge. We could not eat for laughing.” Many of the home stories are concerned with the mistakes of younger children, such as that of the small boy who put the mustard near the open window so that it might get cool ; the girl who was whipped because she had washed the wrong arm when she went to be vaccinated ; the child who, on hearing a cat purr, said, ‘Oh, mother! I think our cat must have swallowed a bee. I can hear it humming inside’; and the girl who refused to sit on her uncle’s knee, because every time he laughed, or drew a deep breath, she was pushed off. Some of the home stories are very popular with the children, as, for example : (a) A young boy said to his father : ‘ Father, I want to marry granny.” His father laughed at him and said, * You can’t marry my mother,” to which he replied, * Why not ? You married mine.’ (b) A boy ran up to his mother and exclaimed, ‘Oh, mother | Baby has swallowed one of my marbles.” The mother was very much distressed and sent for the doctor. To comfort her, the boy said, It’s all right mother, don’t worry, I have some more left.’ (c) A young man, having become engaged to a boy's sister, broke the news to him by saying : ‘ Your sister has x WHAT YOUNG CHILDREN LAUGH AT 103 given herself to me for a Christmas present,” to which the rude boy retorted: ‘Oh! Has she? She gave herself to Mr. Smith last Christmas ; but she was returned to us as an Easter Gift.’ (d) The vicar was asked to have a talk with Nellie, who had been using very bad language. He introduced the subject by saying, ‘ Nellie, I hear that you have been using very bad language.” ‘No, I haven't,” she said, ‘ and who told you?’ ‘A little bird told me,’ said the vicar. Where- upon, Nellie replied, ‘ I know now who it was, it must have been that d—— sparrow I gave the crumbs to this morning.’ With young children stories based on absent-mindedness to which Bergson attaches so much importance as the cause of laughter, are very uncommon. The story of the man who refused to take onions for lunch because his breath would smell, and he had to telephone to a lady friend soon afterwards, is regarded rather as stupidity than absent-mindedness. The same might be said of the lady with a revolver, who found a burglar hanging by his hands on the window sill, and pointing the revolver at him, shouted, ‘hands up’. One genuine case is quoted of a girl who was absorbed in reading an exciting book, when the servant announced that a friend had called to see her. Not wishing to be disturbed, her mother told her sister to go up and tell her friend that she was not at home. But the girl said, * Oh, never mind, I will go and tell her myself,’ which she did, and returned to her book, to the great amusement of her mother and sister. At the age of 12 years, during the period of rapid growth, the type of funny story becomes of a much more extrava- gant nature, and the descriptions of funny sights are affected by the peculiar kind of language employed at this stage of the child’s development. Absurd announcements such as, ‘ Lost ini Kensington : an umbrella, belonging to a gentle- man, with a bent rib,” are frequently quoted. A graphic account is given of a runaway bicycle coming down a 104 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER steep hill. At the foot of the hill the bicyclist seeing a wide plank from the ground to the roof of a house steered for it, and was providentially caught on the other side of the house by people beating carpets in the garden. Such stories as the following, by a boy of 12 years of age, are not uncommon : ; ‘One day when I was on Hampstead Heath, and was flying my toy aeroplane in the air, I heard a constant buzzing noise. It kept on getting louder every moment, till at last I happened to glance upwards and I saw an aeroplane coming towards me. It grew bigger every minute till it came down with a rush and ran along the ground. I wondered where my aeroplane had gone. I looked around and found it right underneath the big one. A costermonger who happened to pass with his wife as the big aeroplane flew upwards, said : ““ Blowed if the big one hasn't laid a little one ”. Even at this age the girls often refer in their stories to the more serious side of life as, for example: In these dark days of unsettlement in the world, there are not many humorous touches with which to cheer the people.’ Reflections of this kind are very rarely found, however, in the boys’ stories. With the cessation of the rapid growth period the stories which provoke laughter revert to a more normal type. CHAPTER X THE LAUGHTER OF OLDER CHILDREN (TWELVE TO EIGHTEEN YEARS OF AGE) children up to the beginning of the period of rapid growth, which may be regarded as, approximately, from the age of eleven and a half to the end of the thirteenth year. This, as we have seen, is a very interesting but somewhat confusing stage in the development of the child. In the earlier years there may be traced a fairly regular continuous growth of the appreciation of humorous situa- tions which cause smiles or laughter. Naturally, as the child acquires further knowledge his interests are increased, the field for the development of humour is enlarged, his taste undergoes a definite change, and he now ceases to laugh at stories and sights which in earlier days caused uproarious merriment. This is more clearly shown in his attitude to funny stories and jokes than to comical sights, which in many cases have a longer range of appeal. In the rapid-growth stage, however, progressive develop- ment can no longer be traced. It is very difficult to prophesy at this time how a boy or girl will respond to a humorous situation. There is frequently a marked change of attitude, and a considerable lowering of the standard of humour, until at last normal growth reappears, and then there is continuous progression as before. The recovery from the serious interference with the cultivation of a \ normal sense of humour appears to come earlier with the girls than the boy. On the other hand, it is probable that the change affected the girls’ attitude to life before the 105 I the last chapter we dealt with the laughter of 106 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER boys’; and, therefore, there is little difference in the actual duration of the break. Even when the rate of growth is restored to the ordinary standard, the after-effect is felt especially in the extravagance of language which is a well-known characteristic of the rapid-growth period. In practice, the springs of laughter during the 14 to 18 years’ stage are comparatively easy of discovery. The progress in the sense of humour is regular, and the effect of the additional elements which enter the field can readily be traced. Whereas, from 12 to 14 years is a stage in which progress, if any, is very difficult to recognize, and it becomes a more or less hopeless task for investigators. A further point which marks off the difference between the laughter of older from that of younger children is that the child has now become a member of a group. The team spirit has entered into his attitude to life. His individualistic outlook has to a certain extent disappeared, and the interests of the self are much affected by his desire to play a part which will secure the good opinion of other members of the group in which he finds himself. This partial transfer of allegiance from those in authority in the school and the home to the social group which has now come into existence, and which will increase as time goes on, naturally colours his response to humorous situations. The consequence is that, quite apart from his advance in knowledge, events of individual interest which form the basis of the material which cause laughter, take a new orientation as the result of the influences at work as to how the group regards the matter in question. . We find, therefore, fundamental changes in the subjects which now cause laughter. Fairy stories have ceased to enjoy the popularity of former days, and are rarely selected in the choice of favourites in connexion with humorous narratives, though, occasionally, a fairy element in the animal or domestic story is, evidently, the turning point for its selection. The misfortunes of others, as the humorous THE LAUGHTER OF OLDER CHILDREN 107 basis, unless concerned with the behaviour of adults, have also practically lost the power of appeal, and even the Punch and Judy Show is rarely mentioned. The critical attitude of the child towards those in authority, which made its appearance at about the age of 10 years, now affects many of the stories, and this increases up to the end of secondary school life. In fact, in the laughter of older children, throughout the whole of the twelve to eighteen period, it is evident that the affairs of grown-up people are of greater interest than those of children. After the difficult period of rapid growth, the selection of humorous stories from good literature increases steadily —both in quantity and quality—during the fourteen to eighteen period. The element of subtlety now becomes apparent in many of the stories, and in some of them it dominates the situation. The most marked change is, however, the contribution of the narrator in describing the incident which causes laughter. The growth of a really good story from the simplest material is interesting in this connexion. This is sometimes the work of successive narrators who improve the humorous effect as it passes from one to the other. On the other hand, an excellent story may be ruined by an incompetent exponent, who has failed to recognize the really humorous points in the story, or because he has not that gift of narration for which many children have great ability. As a rule, where combinations of events which produce hilarious merriment in very young children—such as the boisterous fun of the knockabout variety—have ceased to produce laughter among the older children it is very uncommon for them to recur at a later stage, and produce the original effect. Here and there, however, an old source of laughter may reappear if only for a very limited period. Thus, the riddle, of which the child’s love of the play upon words was the origin, fell off suddenly in popu- larity, and practically disappeared at a comparatively 108 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER early age. For a short time, however—at about the age of 15 to 16 years—the riddle comes to life again in a some- what altered form. It is now associated with class-room material, and the pun is generally used as its foundation. Among those quoted are : (a) A play upon the words Missouri, Mississipi, Delaware, Alaska, and Havana: ‘If Miss Souri lent her jumper to Miss Sissipi, what would Della wear ? I don’t know, but I'll ask her. She might have Anna's.’ (b) A play upon the name Robinson Crusoe: ‘In a poultry yard there were three cocks. One was called Smith, another Brown and the third Robinson. Why was the third called Robinson ? Because he crew so.’ (c) A play upon the words, ‘Come into the garden Maud’: A farmer had three pigs, one was called George, the second John and the third Maud. Why was the third called Maud ? Because she would come into the garden.’ When once a competition is started in riddle formation in a school, at about this age, it will probably enjoy a short season of great popularity ; but the interest soon wanes. This reappearance is not confined to children in English schools ; but, wherever it is found, it is always short-lived, and seems to appeal more to girls than to boys. Throughout the rapid growth stage, however, there are many revivals of more infantile sources of laughter, but this is due to the regression of the standard of humour to that of a lower type during this abnormal period. Stories of exaggeration, which are so characteristic of this phase, retain their popularity with children during the twelve to fourteen stage. References are made to the Japanese version of the Epaminondas folk-story which deals with older children. A boy separates birds who are fighting and is severely pecked. He was told that the appropriate action would have been to stroke the birds and tell them to be friends. Later on he applied this advice, in the case of two fighting bulls, with the inevitable result, and so on. THE LAUGHTER OF OLDER CHILDREN 109 In every way it follows the line of the original story which was so popular with young English and American children. A patient in an asylum kept knocking himself on the head, and, when asked the reason why he did this, replied that it was so pleasant when he left off. This story is fairly typical of this period. Many stories are now told of the frugality of Scotsmen, such, for example, as that of the man who bitterly resented the tipping system in London, which reached the limit when he saw a notice in a lavatory, ‘ Please tip the basin after washing.’ In this connexion an amusing story is quoted of a Scotsman who dropped a shilling in crossing the street, but could not find it because of the heavy traffic. He told a policeman of his loss, and said he would come there the following day to see if it had been found. When he arrived, the street was up for repairs, and he told the policeman that he was very sorry they were taking so much trouble to find his shilling. The ridiculous story is also told of a man who informed his friend that, in taking his bath, he found that a musical box playing in the bathroom added greatly to his enjoyment. The friend thought it was an excellent idea, and bought an old second-hand musical box for his bathroom, but it was a failure, as it could only play ‘ God Save the King’, and, naturally, he had to stand up during the playing of the tune. Other popular stories of exaggeration at this stage are : (@) A man in hospital had just come back to his ward after an operation, and his companions were telling tales of the carelessness of doctors in leaving things, such as forceps, in the body of the patient. The man was becoming some- what alarmed when an attendant came in and said the doctor had lost a small bag and could not find it anywhere. The patient collapsed. ’ (6) The parents of a girl who was engaged to a Scotsman were very indignant because, during a long day in the country with the daughter, he had only contributed half 110 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER a crown towards the expenses of the outing. In order to make him feel uncomfortable, they sent the amount to him at once by the girl’s brother to whom the lover said, ‘ I am sorry you have come with it to-night, to-morrow would have done quite well.’ (c) A little girl feeling very sick in church, her mother told her to go outside until she felt better. Surprised at the child’s quick return the mother asked the reason, and the child explained that, fortunately, she found a box in the porch with the words, ‘ For the sick ’. (d) A man was suffering from sea-sickness on the deck of a ship, and his friend, anxious to be of any assistance, said, ‘I am going down to our cabin, can I bring any- thing up for you?’ ‘No, thank you,’ he replied, ‘ I have no difficulty in that direction.’ (e) A farmer, wanting to buy a bicycle, went into a shop to ask the price of the one on view in the window, and was told that he could buy it for twelve pounds. The farmer laughed and said, ‘ Why I could buy a cow for twelve pounds.” Yes,’ said the shopman, ‘ but you'd look funny riding a cow.” To which the farmer replied, ‘ Not so funny as you would look milking a bicycle.’ The following records of funny sights indicate not only the extravagant language which, as we have seen, is a well-marked feature of this stage of the child’s life, but also his intense dislike of the over-dressed person, who now generally replaces, in describing the misfortunes of others, the fat man or woman of earlier days : (a) ‘ One day in September, passing along our road, I saw an immaculately-dressed foppish young man. His felt hat was tilted to one side, showing long, red, brushed- back hair, and his jacket was resplendent with a purple- coloured handkerchief. He was walking very gingerly along the muddy path when his eye caught sight of the number of a passing bus. “Hi! stop!” he shouted to the conductor. In his excitement, forgetting to examine THE LAUGHTER OF OLDER CHILDREN 111 “every piece of ground before him, he broke into a stilted run. “Come along sir ”, bawled the conductor, pulling the bell vigorously. Without noticing it the gaily-dressed young man unfortunately slid for half a dozen yards on a nicely-placed piece of orange peel. The rest is obvious. Alas for the smart jacket and perfumed handkerchief ! Alas, too, for the small felt hat | It was a muddy hopeless mass of pulp under the hoofs of passing horses. Glaring with watery eyes around him, the no-longer-smart young man picked himself up, and his face was smothered with mud and filth. A crowd of hooligans quickly collected and energetically derided him ; and, cursing the mob, he slowly made his way down a side street, where I was, to my sorrow, in too great a hurry on an errand to follow him.’ (0) ‘ Whilst walking down the road on one Saturday morning, I saw a select-looking taxicab in which were a young couple dressed in their wedding garb. Suddenly a loud roar and grinding noise signalized that the car was out of action. After much conversation another car was telephoned for, but all the cars had been hired, and so another was not available. The church was some miles distant, so that to walk would be a tiresome task. Then the funny sight came. An old labourer’s horse and wagon were put into action, and with the aid of a rope and the horses fittings, the car was towed by the horse to the church.’ (c) ‘I was going along the street soliloquizing upon the drabness of the general outlook, when my reveries were interrupted by melodious sounds, which issued from a very ancient-looking personage. His clothes were very dilapi- dated, and the hat that partly covered his hair was more or less in the form of a telescope, for it had a large hole in the top. His nose was red, very red, why I do not know, perhaps he did, but likely enough it was the cold. Any- how, when he had finished singing ““ Bubbles ”’ in a most 112 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER somniferous manner, accompanying himself with a banjo, he started a refrain, which I had never heard before, but which I am sure I shall never forget. It was this: Just try to picture me On my old mother’s knee, etc. Try as I might, I could not at first picture him on his mother’s knee, for he had an extraordinarily large circum- ference, and I rather felt sorry for his poor old mother. Anyhow, after an effort I managed to see him faintly seated upon his fond parent’s knee, and I do not know why, but the result made me laugh. At length, I gave him twopence, and after a grateful ““ Thanks,” he disappeared into a public-house, into which he had been pouring forth his melody, evidently for the purpose of lubricating his throat preparatory to a fresh musical effort.’ The extravagance of language during the period of rapid growth is less marked in the funny stories of the girls than in those of the boys. In describing street scenes, however, there are evidences of this, as, for example, ‘ We were choking with silent mirth at his questioning look and force of gaiety ’ and ‘I could not withstand my curiosity and hastened to the spot’. In the comparatively few girls’ stories of animals there is no falling off in attributing to them extraordinary intelligence and reasoning power. Thus, ‘ A cat having caught her tail on fire dipped it in some water to putit out’. The girls appear to recover their sense of humour during this period earlier than the boys, and their power of description is far greater. The fact that the girls are about to leave school appears to have a marked effect upon them. They appreciate humorous situations in a more delicate way, and the more boisterous fun attitude is practically non-existent. In the twelve to fourteen period there are considerable differences between the boys and the girls as to the situations which cause laughter. Girls play with thoughts more than THE LAUGHTER OF OLDER CHILDREN 113 boys, and they can build up good stories from very simple foundations. Boys, on the other hand, appear to have a greater range and variety as regards humorous incidents. Girls’ stories are of a more domestic character. The following are typical : (a) © At an evening party they were playing the game of “ Ugly Faces”. A person goes out of the room, and those who remain make grimaces in competition for the prize for the ugliest face, to be awarded by the one outside. One lady, however, refused to play the game, but the judge, unaware of this, on entering the room, awarded her the prize to the great amusement of the others.’ (6) “ A family, going out for the day, put up a notice on the door : ““ All out. Don’t leave anything.” On return- ing, they found that the house had been visited by burglars, who wrote beneath the notice, “ We haven't left much ”’. (¢) “A child was commissioned to buy a mouse-trap among other items of shopping. Pressed for time, she ran into a shop and said, “ A mouse-trap, please, make haste as I want to catch a tram In the girls’ stories there is frequently a tendency to attach a moral, but this is rarely found in those of the boys. Another marked difference is that the point of the girls’ story very frequently concerns the stupidity of a boy ; but the converse scarcely ever occurs. An opportunity was afforded of investigating the sense of humour of a large number of children who had recently entered employment on leaving school at the age of 14 years. Many interesting changes are to be observed as the result of the great alteration of environment. The most marked feature is the rapid development of the superiority element in the attitude of the boys towards humorous situations. They are no longer subject to the same type of authority, and the greater freedom, accompanied by the necessity of forming judgments for themselves, all affect the sense of humour. The objects which produce mirth 8 114 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER are now practically all concerned with adults who have become more interesting than children. The descriptions of funny sights are still somewhat florid, but the excessive use of adjectives takes the place, to a certain extent, of extravagant expressions. The children are more critical, and poke fun at adults in positions of authority. A curious change is that the boys are becoming more introspective, and have a keener appreciation of details, and thus enter into competition with the girls who formerly were far in advance in this respect. The girls, at this stage, do not develop along the line of increasing superiority. They are still interested in stories of children. Dreams are quoted frequently as the funniest sights they have seen, and they appear to day-dream far more than the boys. Girls also moralize more than boys ; they talk round a subject, and occasionally discuss the value of humour. Practically for the first time, they laugh at stupid things they have done themselves, and cases are not infrequent of stories of absent-mindedness. A favourite story in this connexion is that of a man who had a room over that of a woman who was seriously ill, and he promised to make no noise. In a fit of absent-mindedness and irritation, on taking off his boots, he threw one of them across the room, and then remembered his promise. After some time the nurse of the invalid came up and asked him when he was going to take off the other boot. Animal stories are far less common with older children, and these are, moreover, of a somewhat different type. Thus, a child gives an account of the infliction of corporal punishment in the animal world : ‘ At last Tiger (the cat) caught Blackie (the kitten) and she took hold of his ear with her teeth, and, with her paws, she gave him a thorough good smacking.” Another story suggests admirable human qualities : ‘ Our dog made friends with the kitten because she resembled her mother who had been a great friend of his.” Another function of the Zoo is referred to in one of THE LAUGHTER OF OLDER CHILDREN 115 the animal stories : ‘ Some people delight in making fun of the blundering mistakes of foreigners. This shows a lack of sympathy. I think nature provides us with far more comical scenes and the best place for this is the Zoo’. A child also tells a story of seeing a clever elephant who accepted half-crowns from visitors, and placed them in a money box on a high shelf ; but the attendant explained that the elephant had never been taught how to take them out again. The following records of animal stories show clearly the growth of the power of the narrator to describe the simplest situations in such a manner as to produce interesting stories. They are arranged in order of the age of the children : (a), (b), and (c) belong to the 12 to 14, and (d), and (e) to the 14 to 18 period. (a) ‘I was sitting by the fire reading a book when a knock came at the front door. By looking out of a certain window in our house you can see our front door, so I looked out, and there was the cat standing on the step on his hind legs and with one of his front paws was knocking at the door. Then I went and let him in. I think this is the funniest sight I have ever seen.’ (b) ‘ Our two cats were sleeping in a box with a lot of papers on them, and pieces of wood. Happening in my conversation to mention the word ‘‘ meat ’’, there was a sudden clatter and the cats came flying out, knocking over the box and wood, yet they had been apparently fast asleep! This proves that cats are always alert even in their sleep.’ (¢) ‘ This happened one day, when I was reading a book out in the garden. An empty condensed milk tin was carelessly thrown in the direction of a dust-bin. It was intended to go in but fell short. Our little Persian cat immediately rushed at it, and being a little playful began to play with it. After about a minute’s play, noticing a little milk in it, it began scraping out the contents. It 116 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER had nearly scraped it all out when in an effort to lick the bottom of the tin it wedged its head in it. Instead of trying to get it off the cat raced round the garden, and three or four times banged against a wall, jamming it on much tighter. I could not get at it for as soon as it heard my feet it ran in another direction. I managed to get the cat after chasing it for several minutes and when I took the tin off its head it jumped on my shoulders and licked my face.’ (@) © A friend of mine kept two ducks and a very fat old rabbit in her garden. These three had a free run of the garden, which was quite a large one, and the two ducks though of different breed became fast friends. Although the rabbit was not on such good terms he was very friendly from the start. One day, feeling mischievous I suppose, one duck, whom we will call Billy, went up to the rabbit who was sitting in the middle of the garden. After he had waddled near enough Billy gave the rabbit a dig in the back with his bill. This had no effect on the rabbit, who I suppose was used to such behaviour. Billy then went back to the other duck, and must have planned with her, in “ Duckish ” I suppose, to play a trick on Bunny. They both waddled up to the rabbit who, though he saw them coming, remained seated. They went one on each side and then deliberately took hold of an ear each and pulled it. This of course was too much to put up with and so Bunny thumped his tail and lopped off. The two ducks stood where they were and quacked. I suppose it was their interpretation of a laugh.’ (e) ‘ At home we have a big dog named Jack. One day I was out walking and I had Jack by my side. He is a big dog with a very fierce disposition but he is very chivalrous to lady dogs and cats. As I was walking past a house a very tiny kitten fell out of the front door, there is really no other word for it. It seemed to rollout like a very tiny ball of fur. This ball came to a stop, uncurled THE LAUGHTER OF OLDER CHILDREN 117 itself and came upright, eyes bright and tail up. The kitten was on the war-path, I could see that and she seemed to resent Jack being in the neighbourhood of her house. As I watched she came to a decision and decided that Jack must go. Well, the next minute this atom of energy had hurled itself at the dog’s heels and began scratching and fighting for dear life. Of course Jack ran to get out of the way of this tiny kitten. With big strides he made a wide detour and came back to me. After him went the kitten, all four tiny legs going like piston rods, as fast as she could. Each time Jack stopped so the kitten just reached him and he had to start off again. Of course he had too much chivalry to hurt it, but the kitten was bent on hurting him. The look on the dog’s face was such a mixture of hurt dignity, annoyance and boredom that I laughed till I could laugh no longer. At last we evidently got far enough away for the kitten to decide that she had done her duty and off she went her tail high in the air, with a mincing step as though she had conquered an army.’ The stories and descriptions of sights of the streets, trams, buses and trains are, naturally, associated with the laughter of older as with that of younger children. Here, as with other sources of laughter common to both periods, we find that from 12 to 14 there is a much closer connexion between the springs of laughter referred to in the last chapter, whereas, there is a marked difference after leaving the rapid-growth period. As the sense of humour develops in the narrator, and a greater power of literary expression manifests itself, we find that the method of narration frequently atones for the poverty of the basal humorous situation. In this connexion, the descriptive power of the girl, especially at the ages of 14 and 15, is notably superior to that of the boy, who has to rely more upon the intrinsic merits of the original material whether in story or in the comical sight. In both, as we have seen, the greater interest taken in the behaviour of the adult is 118 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER to be noticed, but it is stronger in the boy than the girl who still discovers in the doings of children occasions for laughter. The animal performing in the street is a delight to the child : ‘ The dance which made the people laugh the most was the military two-step of the clever dog who heel-toed so nicely, and strutted up and down like a soldier, until the children screamed with laughter.” Children, with their dislike of people who are too elaborately dressed, generally take the side of the working man in any alterca- tion. When a swell in a railway carriage complained to the porter that he did not sing out the name of the station properly, and he replied that he was not a * Hopera singer ’, the youthful portion of the company rejoiced, and one of them gave an account of it as a good story. The boy, in his accounts of interviews with officials, frequently describes the incidents in extravagant language as, for example, ‘ When a policeman said I wouldn’t arrest a kid like you, I looked at him with hauteur and rose and marched off with dignity.” Another boy being suspicious of the behaviour of a tramp says: ‘I decided to act the part of a sleuth-hound and, with this idea firmly implanted in my mind, I followed him.’ The superiority element in humour is frequently the dominant feature in street scenes, as in a description of the long time it took a man to see the point of the joke in the advertisement of Watney’s stout (What knees), whereas the recorder of the sight saw it at once. And, again, the amusement caused by the stupid boy who had put a six- pence into a chocolate slot machine, and was waiting patiently for the change and the chocolate. A favourite story in this connexion is that of a dog whose lead was fastened to an umbrella. The dog, seeing a cat, broke away, taking the umbrella with him, and caused minor accidents in the street to the great amusement of the children onlookers. THE LAUGHTER OF OLDER CHILDREN 119 The tram conductor in his disputes with passengers is a fertile source of laughter : (2) The two aggrieved women who keep the occupants of the tram in a state of hilarity by their references to the conductor’s beautiful face and kindly manners. (b) The woman who asked the conductor to let her know when she reached a certain point and, when he stopped the tram, said she only wanted to show her little dog, Fido, the house where he was born. And, (c) The woman whose child (George) is crying, and the conductor calls out George’s Cross, to which the woman retorted, thinking a reference was being made to her boy, ‘ Yes, and so would you be if you were cutting your teeth’. Such incidents are occasions of much merriment for the children. As in the case of the animal stories, those of street incidents also illustrate the great difference between the earlier records of the children and those of the 14 to 18 period. This is well shown in the following examples: (4) and (b) are crowded with details of the sights seen with very little addition by the narrators. Whereas, in the cases of (c) and (d), each has a very slender basis in actual occurrence, and is made up almost entirely of the contribution of the narrator : (a) ‘ Our suburb recently noted with interest a queer couple accompanied by a pram parading the streets. People who were hurrying to business, and others who were whiling away the time, all stared curiously at the figures of Charlie Chaplin and ““ The Kid ”’. People who did not care for the picture which created so much interest couldn’t help being amused at the ridiculous antics of “ Charlie Chaplin ”. When ‘ Charlie ”’ raised his hat to a minute stone which he fell over the children chorused with laughter. Grown-ups in the trams and buses smiled when “ Charlie ” raised his hat backwards and ‘‘ The Kid ” pushed the pram. He peered into people’s baskets with a wistful face, twirled his tiny cane and hooked the neck of 120 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER a delighted youngster in front. Not very often do things like this happen and one may be assured that the younger generation see to it that they get all the enjoyment they can out of them.’ (b) ‘It was on a cool summer evening that my cousin and I were strolling along the banks of the river Thames. We sat down for some time on a little patch of grass, when two or three joyful barks attracted our attention, and much to our amusement we saw a little boy, who had evidently come from the swimming baths, bewildered by a little terrier who had got hold of the boy’s towel. He tried hard for some time to release the towel from the dog’s mouth, but the harder he pulled the harder the dog pulled. The little dog was about to give up when he gave a final pull, and, much to his glee, he was sole possessor of the towel. We started to chase him, and a gentleman seeing what had happened ran forward to release the towel ; this he did, and gave it to the boy who afterwards went home. This was not all, for when the gentleman turned to pick up his stick he found the dog had it. The little dog was very artful, he would put the stick down, and he would wait till we were near him and then run away again. At last his attention wandered elsewhere and after a few minutes the man had his stick again. We laughed over this for many days after, and never forgot the innocent expression on the face of the dog.’ (¢) ‘ The funniest sight I have ever seen was that of an old gentleman chasing his hat. I do not know why, but this spectacle always seems to amuse the onlookers. Perhaps it is because the crowd is always pleased to see the mighty fallen. A moment before, this portly old gentleman, with red face and bristling whiskers, had strode along like some conqueror of old. Clerks and typists seemed to hurry from his path, while newsboys had insinuatingly held forth their papers as though they were some humble petition. Then along came the North THE LAUGHTER OF OLDER CHILDREN 121 Wind, who respects nobody, and in a moment all was changed.’ ‘The gentleman's top hat, casting all dignity to the winds, bounded from the stately head of the wearer and, fluttering to the ground, began to bowl merrily along the street. The gentleman, after one frantic clutch to save it, doubled up in the proper athletic style, and charged after it head foremost. There they went down the street, the hat bounding and leaping and seeming possessed with the very spirit of mischief ; the gentleman tearing after it, his face becoming redder and redder, coat-tails flying and little legs going like clock-work. Now and then he would bend nearly double, thinking he had it, but the hat just managed to leap ahead, and the gentleman, after nearly tripping over, would plunge ahead once more. On his way he was accompanied by the wind’s shriek of glee, and the laughter of the bystanders. He was encouraged by yells of, “ Go it Guv’'nor ”’ and “ Put yer beef into it,” while another youth was willing to have, “ Two to one on the 'at ”’. Thus they went, tearing away, the sport of the North Wind, growing smaller and smaller until they turned the corner and disappeared from view.’ (d) ‘It has always struck me as surprising and quite inexplicable that a grown man, in all other respects possibly above the average intelligence and capacity, should persistently be late for his train in the morning. That such a logical creature as the London business speculator should think it worth while to sacrifice his dignity and comfort in his journey to the station for a few more short moments in bed is a quite unexpected departure from the paths of reason.’ ‘And perhaps it is merely because of its unnaturalness that the sight of a procession of middle-aged, well-groomed men and women, with set faces and clenched fists, proceed- ing at a steady jog-trot towards their suburban railway station (what time the fiery dragon of steel, belching forth 122 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER its breath of fire in contempt, pursues its relentless course along the iron road to the metropolis some hundred yards in the rear) appears to me the most truly funny of the many sights which our city affords.’ ‘Take this man, for example, now sprinting with renewed vigour as the station bell clangs out its warning—he is perhaps the controlling power of a seething hive of business activity ; he sits all day in his Holy of Holies, supplying solutions to momentous problems and performing quite extraordinary feats of mental skill and initiative—and yet, during those few minutes preceding the time of departure of his train, he is reduced to the level of the meanest city clerk.’ The laughter of older children is concerned only to a very slight extent with country scenes. The glorious novelty of rural life is wearing off after many expeditions, and the stories told are, as a rule, not of personal experiences but of absurd events recorded of younger children, such as that of the boy who was fascinated by the sight of a cow being milked. To his delight, he found a porcelain cow on the mantelpiece in his bedroom and he spent much time in looking at it. He had nightmare, and was found sobbing as he tried in vain to milk the little cow. There are many variants of the story of the faithful dog who has charge of his master’s clothes while he is bathing. Here, the clothes are attached to the dog who, feeling anxious about his master’s safety, swims out to bring him ashore. The most extravagant story recorded of country life is that of the boy of fourteen who describes a prize fight between two robins in a ring surrounded by a hundred robins intently watching the contest. The stories of school life now take a far more important place than with the younger children. The following are examples: (a) A teacher was surprised that a boy who, in answer to a question as to the meaning of the word furlough, replied, ‘a mule’, and found that the THE LAUGHTER OF OLDER CHILDREN 128 misunderstanding arose from a picture he had seen of a soldier riding on a mule with the title, ‘ Going home on furlough’. (b) A child, who had never heard of monogamy before, after a lesson on marriage customs, astonished the class by saying in reply to a question, ‘ When a man is a Christian, and has only one wife, it is called monotony ’. (c) Hearing, at a prize distribution, that a pious donor had given large sums to the school, a boy remarked, ‘ Why couldn’t he do them himself.’ At a later stage the * Hamlet ’ story is frequently referred to. An imspector was examining a class on English literature, and finding one boy very stupid asked him in a loud voice, * Who wrote Hamlet ? ’ to which the boy replied, ‘ Please, sir, I didn’t.” The inspector was much amused, and, at night, when he was dining with a farmer in the neighbourhood, he told the story. The farmer did not see the point and remarked, with a smile, “ Young rascal, I expect he did’. The delight in commenting severely on class-room events is well illustrated by the description of a girl of 17 years of age on a singing lesson : ‘On the platform a bright cheerful lady stood with a silver-topped baton in her hand. After commanding silence she took her seat at the piano and suddenly the beautiful chords of a well-known song sounded through the hall. After a few bars of perfectly harmonized music had been played through in a most martial manner the sound of weird, melancholy and raucous voices tore through the atmosphere like the piercing shrieks of a thousand sea-gulls over the rugged cliffs of an unknown shore.’ Stories about people in authority who are in any way held up to ridicule are exceptionally popular with boys and girls during the 14 to 18 period: (2) The witness in court who said that the man of whom he was ~ speaking was ‘ as drunk as a judge ’, and was corrected by the judge who said, ‘ You mean as drunk as a lord’. ‘I beg your pardon, my lord,” he replied. (b) At a council 124 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER meeting, the mayor who was impressed by the excellent service rendered by a subordinate official suggested that he should be given a honorarium. Whereupon, a Labour member retorted, ‘ What's the good of that, he wouldn’t be able to play it, let’s give him five pounds.’ Occurrences in church are also fertile sources of favourite stories, some of them being of an extravagant character, as, for example, that of the church in which an additional font had been provided, ‘ so that in future babies might be christened at both ends’, and, again, in an announcement about egg-Sunday the parishioners were requested, ‘to lay their eggs in the font’. A curate complained to a friend of his embarrassment at the attention he received from so many importunate young ladies of the church, and was told that there was no need to worry as there was ‘ safety in numbers’, to which he replied, there might be even greater ““ safety in exodus”. Many stories are told of language used in the pulpit which might have a double meaning, as that in which a clergyman, revisiting his former church in the Isle of Wight, expressed his joy at seeing before him again ‘so many old Cowes faces’. Another clergyman, who was much touched by the kind- ness of his parishioners in sending him a birthday present of brandy cherries, referred to it on the following Sunday morning, and said. * What pleased me most was the spirit in which it was sent’. Yet, another favourite story is that of the Jew who fainted in church during the collection and was carried out by two Scotsmen. | During the 14 to 18 period the children have recovered from the stage of rapid growth—in which there was such a serious deterioration of the sense of humour —and now fairly general progress is made all along the line. At the age of 15 years, the appreciation of the ludicrous appears to reach its highest point as the cause of laughter and then gives way in competition with other elements of humour. Fortunately, however, in the normal person it is THE LAUGHTER OF OLDER CHILDREN 125 never entirely lost and may be frequently a great source of comfort in many difficult situations. As we have already seen, as time goes on the part played by the simplest humorous situation may result in an excellent story, or in the vivid description of a funny sight. The appearance of subtlety greatly increases the range of laughter-provoking material, and educational achievement naturally enlarges the area for the play of the spirit of humour. During the later part of school life there are many excellent stories told of the use of formulas which are im- perfectly understood. The critical attitude which as we have found, makes its appearance in the child at about the tenth year, now takes on a useful form, and laughter acts as a deterrent to the student who professes a higher standard of knowledge than he can rightly claim. In this way laughter performs a most valuable function. In an investigation of the sense of humour of a large number of children, it is interesting to notice the remarkable variation from age to age, and the difference in the type of situation which appeals to boys and girls. Up to the age of 16 or 17 years, the girls’ greater power of description, and probably a keener sense of humour, give them the premier position. But towards the close of higher school life the boys advance very rapidly and in originality of thought, and especially in the greater variety of treat- ment, they would appear to have the advantage. The amount of material available, however, is not sufficient upon which to come to anything in the nature of a definite conclusion. CHAPTER XI ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR ’ : ‘HE fundamental sense of humour, in spite of many statements to the contrary, is much the same in England and America. The material on which it operates, however, differs widely, and thus we get a clear distinction between English and American humour. There are various definitions of what is termed American when contrasted with English humour, and most of them stress the distinguishing feature that, in the former greater emphasis is placed on one or more of the elements of exaggeration, gross absurdity and mendacity in laughter- provoking situations. In this connexion it may be well to recall Mark Twain’s description: ‘To bring incon- gruities and absurdities together in a wandering purposeless way and seem innocently unaware that they are absurdities is the basis of the American art.’ The nature of the environment, the social outlook, and the advance of civilization must, of necessity, have a profound effect on the things we laugh at. And nothing illustrates this better than the change of attitude from cen- tury to century and the folly, of which so much advantage has been taken, of discussing the statements of experts who have written on humour and laughter from age to age under widely different social conditions. The definition of a couple of centuries ago, which was then perfectly sound, naturally fails to bear careful examination success- fully in the altered outlook of to-day. A study of the material which causes laughter in peoples of different stages of culture make this abundantly clear. The 126 ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 127 ~ humanization of laughter accompanies the progress of higher ideals in national life. As we have seen, in discussing the sudden-glory theory of Hobbes, there has been, for example, an extraordinary change in our attitude towards deformities in drawing the line between the things which can, and those which cannot, decently be the subject of merriment in dealing with the ~ misfortunes of others. It stands to reason that as environ- ment, history and tradition have such a great effect on the laughter-provoking material of a people, there must ~ be a considerable difference between English and American humour. A new country with a comparative absence of tradition, and therefore with every opportunity for great freedom of expression, must have a different type of humour from that of an old country bound by tradition and with a people anxious above all things to keep well within social conventions. In America the professional humorists have had an abnormal effect on the favourite subjects for laughter. This point has not been sufficiently recognized, and, as it is a matter of supreme importance in comparing English with American humour, it becomes necessary to make a brief study of what may be termed the Mark Twain school as there has never been a corresponding movement in England. The great triumvirate which formed the Mark Twain school consisted of Artemus Ward born in 1834, Mark Twain in 1835, and Bret Harte in 1839. These dominant figures, with an age range of only five years, would undoubtedly have, in any case, produced a marked effect on the future of American humour. But the fact that for many years they were closely associated in literary work, and on the lecture platform, had the result of bringing into existence a well-defined school of humour which made a profound impression on the American people, the in- fluence of which persists, and is clearly in evidence, in the American schools of to-day. 128 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER Artemus Ward was the great °Mis-speller’. He preferred to write phonetically, and this probably somewhat interfered with the appreciation of his literary work in this country. His first contributions appeared in a comic weekly at Boston, the Carpet Bag, in connexion with which he was a compositor. Later on he became a member of the great literary circle at San Francisco, of which Bret Harte was for a long time the president. In America and England he acquired great fame as a lecturer of considerable distinction, and he influenced Mark Twain in his style of lecturing. His humour was genial, kind and never pro- fane, and he was the most lovable of men. He came to England and became a noted contributor to Punch, under Mark Lemon. It is reported that John Bright went to one of his lectures, and said afterwards, that he had enjoyed it very much but that ‘he doubted some of Artemus Ward's statements’. To the great regret of a large circle of English and American friends Artemus Ward died at the early age of 34. Bret Harte, the scholar, dreamer and poet, was for a considerable time one of the greatest, if not the greatest, short-story writer in the world. He had a wonderful style and practically every sentence was a little gem. Mark Twain came under his influence in the literary circle at San Francisco, which had the happy effect of improving his style of writing immensely. The debt he owed to Bret Harte was freely acknowledged by Mark Twain. In The Vulgar Little Boy there is a good example of his concen- tration, in the retort of the boy who had been playfully lashed by a whip by a passing driver. ‘A single sentence conveyed in a few words a reflection on the legitimacy Jf the driver's birth, it hinted a suspicion of the father’s integrity and impugned the fair name of his mother. It suggested incompetence in his present position, personal uncleanliness and evinced a doubt as to his future salva- tion.” This example was quoted by Bret Harte in his ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 129 lectures in England. He also told the story of a boy who was reproved by an old lady for swearing, to which the boy replied, ‘ Call that swearin’, Mum. You should hear our Bill exhort the impenitent mule.’ The turning point, financially and otherwise, in Bret Harte’s career was the publication in the Overland Monthly, of which he was editor, of The Luck of Roaring Camp. The verdict was instantaneous. The story was hailed in Boston as a classic, and he was offered $10,000 a year to become a regular contributor to the Atlantic Monthly. His genius in depicting boy-life is clearly shown in such stories as Maiggles, and in his lecturing he showed his great preference for his efforts in dealing with children. In many of his stories the influence of Dickens, of whom he was a profound admirer, is clearly seen. The character sketches in The Outcasts of Poker Flat are singularly powerful and effective. This is considered by some to be of higher merit in many ways than The Luck of Roaring Camp. He spent the latter part of his life in Europe and acted as Consul at Crefeld and Glasgow. Mark Twain, as he was known after definitely entering literary life, had as a boy and young man a series of remark- able experiences which formed an excellent background and material for many of his books. After assisting his brother in the office of the Hannibal Journal, as printer and assistant editor, he became a licensed pilot on the Mississippi, then a gold-miner and after that took a position as a news- paper reporter. Artemus Ward recognized his ability and helped him forward, and at the age of twenty-nine he came under Bret Harte’s influence at the famous literary circle. The success of a story in Harper's Magazine encouraged him to write his first book, The Jumping Frog. He was now attracting favourable attention as a humorist lecturer and was on the high road to success. Fhe Innocents Abroad, written from material obtained during a five months’ tour in the Holy Land, was a great literary and 9 130 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER financial success and firmly established him in public favour. Other books of travel followed: Roughing It, A Tramp Abroad, Life on the Mississippi, and Following the Equator. In fiction he achieved extraordinary popularity with Huckleberry Finn, Tom Sawyer, and, to a lesser extent, with A Connecticut Yankee in the Court of King Arthur, and The Prince and the Pauper. A book, Joan of Arc, which was first published anonymously is on an entirely different basis. It is a fine panegyric, represents a certain amount of careful research, and gave Mark Twain great pleasure in its production. His fame as a lecturer was unbounded, and when, to meet financial difficulties caused by unsuccessful business ventures, he went on a world lecturing tour and achieved a remarkable success, and effectively disposed of the difficulty which had arisen. To the end he retained his delightful sense of humour, and when at the age of 72 he came to England to receive an honorary degree con- ferred upon him by the University of Oxford, and a rumour to the effect that he was dead was circulated, the king of laughter-makers made the whole world laugh again by his reassuring message to America, ‘ The reports of my death have been grossly exaggerated.’ The short sketch given of the Mark Twain School, with its enormous publicity through its literature and the lecturing platform, sufficiently indicates the elements which gave a very definite stamp to American humour. In addition to this, the rapid growth and extraordinary prosperity of America have produced a fertile soil for the development of a habit of exaggeration. The English humorist makes comparatively little use of exaggeration and gross absurdities ; it would not be tolerated and he has, therefore, to obtain his humour effect from other sources. Whereas, a humorist, in a comparatively young country, with his greater freedom of expression, together ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 131 with a more or less standardized type of humour, is on safer ground. On the other hand, the absence of any authoritative school, or recognized type of humour, gives the opportunity for a more individualistic treatment of laughter-producing material, in consequence of the greater variety in view-point of the humorist, provided that he does not trespass beyond the bounds of the conventional social attitude of his readers. Such delightful stories, for example, as those of : (2) The walking tour and the long search for the lost sock at the end of which the pedometer registered forty-seven miles. (0) The man who proposed to travel from the Gorner Grat to Zermatt by locating himself on the central part of the glacier, and putting his luggage on the sides to come down by slow freight, and (¢) The lake which went down two inches when the big fish was landed, are intensely amusing. These stories from an English writer would meet with little success, but we rock with laughter when they come from Mark Twain. English humour is less primitive in its nature, but the charge is made that it frequently becomes too subtle, heavy, and pedantic. There is less boisterous fun in English than in American humour ; the play element is less dominant. We are, however, apt to take ourselves much too seriously, and it is a genuine relief to turn occasionally to Tom Sawyer and Huckleberry Finn to restore our mental balance. In my investigation of the sense of humour of American children, with the kind co-operation of American teachers, a very large number of selected funny stories, jokes and descriptions of funny sights, were obtained from the schools in answer to the same questions that had been given to the London children. In each case the age of the boy or girl was stated, so that it became possible to compare the type of humorous situation selected by large groups of children of the same age in both countries. It is of special interest to notice that though the order of appearance of well- 132 2 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER marked humorous types, as evidenced by selected stories and descriptions of funny sights, is much the same, the age at which they appear, however, varies considerably, as does also the duration of the special appeal. The effect of the Mark Twain school of humour is particu- larly well shown, not only in the large percentage of stories at different ages of the exaggeration and gross absurdities types, but also in the great number of stories indirectly influenced by the writings of Mark Twain. In addition, there are others which are unmistakable variants of actual stories recorded by him, as will be seen in the illustrations to be given later on. In one large group no less than twenty-three per cent of the stories were traceable to this source. Among English children exaggeration stories are confined almost entirely to the ages of 12 and 13 years— that is during the period of rapid growth—but with the American children exaggeration is practically a stock form of humour from 11 to 18; generally far more among boys than girls. At the upper ages, however, it is very prominent among the girls’ stories. In connexion with the play upon words the stories of the American children of funny mistakes are naturally very numerous, especially at the ages of 8 to 11. This is due to the great mixture of races and the difficulty of children in making themselves understood in, to them, a foreign language. There is a marked falling off of stories of this type at 12 years of age, but they reappear three years later in a different form. It was seen that with English children the riddle was the crowning point of the young child’s delight in playing with words at the ages of 8 and 9. In America the riddle appears much later and is never so popular as in England, as it is frequently asso- ciated with puns which, fortunately, have never taken firm hold in American literature. We have seen among English children that the appeal of the riddle makes a short-lived reappearance at the ages of 15 and 16, and, curiously ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 133 enough, with American boys and girls there is a similar reawakening of a vivid interest in playing with words at the same ages. This revival of interest in a special type of mental attitude, if based on an adequate amount of material, must have some significance though it may be difficult to explain the cause. But where, in a separate investigation, the same phenomenon is to be observed, it assumes a position of greater importance. In this connexion, it should be noted that though there are marked differences in many respects in the type of humorous situation which makes a strong appeal to both English and American children yet, in a variety of important points there is a striking similarity. Thus, in the earlier stages, the mis- fortunes of others become an interesting basis for humorous material, but this type ceases to appeal at about the age of 10 years in both countries. When it takes the form of criticism of authority, in the delight of boys and girls in the mistakes of prominent officials, it is on an entirely different footing. : ~The element of superiority in humour, which finds ‘expression in stories of stupidity first in that of mistakes of younger children and later in those of adults, is more fully developed in American than in English schools. There are more than twice as many stories of stupidity selected by American children at nearly all ages. For example, at 12 years of age the percentage of stories of this kind is about twenty for both boys and girls in America, whereas, for English boys the percentage is fourteen, and for English girls it is only eight, although this is the great age for this type of story in English schools. The explana- tion may be due to some extent to the greater freedom of the American child in expressing his own opinion at a much earlier stage than the English child. The six to ten period of docility, with its greater subservience to authority, is less marked in American schools, with the result that 134 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER the critical attitude of the child makes its appearance long before it is developed in English schools. It thus appears from the investigation that one of the greatest differencesis that stories and jokes of gross absurdity are far more common with American children, and that there are far fewer of stories involving the smart retort, and those included under ‘surprise followed by a relief from restraint’. At the ages of 13 and 14 the per- centages of stories of absurdity selected by boys and girls are as twenty-six to thirty-eight and thirty to forty- five respectively. At 12 years of age, however, the boys lead in the selection of this type of story, the percentages being twenty-six to twenty. Throughout the whole of school life, as shown by the above percentages, the appeal which stories of gross absurdity makes to American children is quite abnormal. The popularity of the fairy story with English children reaches its maximum at about 9 years of age. It is still very strong at 10; but after that this element is negligible with the boys; but the girls—always much keener on fairy stories than the boys—select about ten per cent of their favourite stories from this source up to the age of 16. In the early stages fairy and animal stories are even more popular with American than with English children. If there is, in the story, in addition to the fairy element, a humorous situation of gross absurdity its popularity is assured, and it will probably find a place in the selected list. The great appeal of the animal stories to the American child finds expression in the extraordinary, and well-deserved, popularity of such books as those in the Dr. Dolittle series. There is a wide difference between the favourite domestic story of the American and that of the English child. The books for American children from 8 to 12 years of age receive very great attention, so that readers for each year have not only appropriate material but also the method of ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 185 presentation which is regarded as being most suitable for the particular age of the boy or girl. This standardization of picture book might be severely criticized, but it serves its purpose admirably, and there is no question whatever that such books make the desired appeal most effectively. The result is that stories of the domestic order are, with the American child, largely based upon these books. Whereas, with the English child they more rarely come from literary sources, but are mainly descriptions of comical incidents which have interested the children in the home. Many also are records of funny stories they have heard. It is astonishing to find what a large proportion of funny stories are the common possession of English and American children. Up to the age of 12 years the number is some- where in the neighbourhood of twenty per cent. Above that age the percentage is not so high; the stories have now much less in common and have interesting characteris- tics of their own. The contribution of the narrator, to which many references have been made, is not so much in evidence in American as in English stories. The play element dominates the situation to the end of the school course. Here are two stories selected by girls of 17 years of age. One deals with the life of man and the other with an extravagant story of two travellers. (a) ‘ The life of man. When he comes into the world every one wants to kiss him, before he goes out they all want to kick him. If he dies young there was a great future before him. If he lives to a ripe old age he is simply in the way and living to save funeral expenses.’ (6) ‘ Two travellers, for whom there was not room in the hotel, had beds fixed up for them in a neighbouring church. The town was alarmed early in the morning by the church bell ringing. There was great consternation and a man rushed in from the hotel. “Iced water for pew 25,” said the man who had rung the bell.’ . 136 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER Books of children’s stories are frequently published in both countries, regardless of the place of their origin. Some of these become popular and pass from school to school ; others make no appeal and are rarely quoted. As a rule, however, provided that the story does not depend for its value in causing laughter on local incidents or slang phrases, great popularity in one country generally finds a cordial response in the other. Just as revues, farces and comedies which have had long runs in America generally do equally well in England and vice versa. In the case of occasional and sometimes very notable, failures, the reason may be found in the stressing of some particular element in humorous appeal which misses the necessary stimulus in the psychology of a different type of audience. It is abnormally difficult to form a reliable estimate of public approval before an actual test. The same is true of the success of failure of funny stories or jokes. It is interesting to find that some professions appear to provide more material for the basis of children’s stories than others. There are many dentists’ stories: (a) A man suggested an improvement on the term dental parlour, and proposed calling it a drawing-room. An Englishman who professed to be unable to see the point of this suggestion was charged with having no sense of humour. (5) Asked why he had had the fire escape removed from his room, a dentist replied that he had lost twelve patients in that way. (¢) A boy went to have a tooth extracted, and the dentist told him that it would be seventy-five cents if he screamed, but only fifty cents if he was a good boy. Asked by his mother if he had screamed, he replied, ‘I couldn’t, you only gave me half a dollar’. Similarly, there are many stories about doctors: A man called up a doctor because he couldn't get off to sleep. The doctor said, ‘ Hold the wire and I will sing you a lullaby.” Another man went to see a doctor about the ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 137 state of his health, who found that he was suffering from a serious loss of memory. ‘And what did the doctor do,’ asked his friend. ‘Oh,’ he replied, ‘ he made me pay his bill in advance.’ Two stories, frequently quoted in different countries, are very popular with children. The first is that of the university professor who was also a skilled surgeon. One morning, for the information of his students, he put on the notice board, ‘I have to perform an operation on the king and will return presently’. Underneath the notice a student wrote, ‘ God save the king’. The other is that of a doctor who was attempting to persuade a student to give up alcohol as it was injuring his health. ‘On the contrary doctor,” he said, ‘I can prove that it is making me much stronger, for the last time I had a cask of beer sent to my room I was so weak that I could hardly move it, but before it had been there a month I could carry it round my room three times without any difficulty.’ It was seen, in dealing with the stories of English children, that the origin of many of them was concerned with the doings of clergymen, or with incidents happening in church. Those of the American children are of a rather different type as will be seen by the following examples: (a) A baby was perfectly good during the christening ceremony. He just smiled all the way through. The clergyman was so much impressed that he congratu- lated the parents on the baby’s splendid behaviour. ‘ Thank you, sir,” said the mother. ‘ Jim and I have been practising on him with a pail of water for the last ten days.” (b) A priest, having asked a boy the way to the Post Office, invited him to come and hear him preach on the following Sunday and he would show him the way to heaven ‘Go on,’ said the boy, ‘ you don’t even know the way to the Post Office.” (There are many variants of this story.) (c) A missionary with a cork leg was captured by cannibals who decided to kill him. He suggested to the 138 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER king, however, that it might be well to sample him before he died. He assented to this, and, having tried to masticate two small pieces from his leg, the missionary was reprieved by the king and became a power in the land. The sea, and affairs on board ship, form the background of many stories: (a) A sailor had been given charge of two valuable laughing jackasses on a ship coming home from Australia. He was much distressed one morning to find that they were both dead, and he broke the sad news to the captain by saying, ‘ Them birds ain’t got nothing to laugh about this morning.” (b) A man, having fallen overboard, the captain shouted to an Irishman to throw out two lifebuoys, but instead of doing so he threw out two boys. ‘I meant cork buoys, you idiot,” said the captain, to which the sailor replied, * How was I to know whether they came from Cork or Dublin.” (c) A lady lost her wig as a sailor swam out to save her from drowning, and she cried out, ‘ Save my hair,’ but the man said, ‘ No, madam, I am a life-saver not a hair-restorer.” (d) A lady on board ship asked the captain whether the equator could be seen. ‘ Yes, madam,’ he said, ‘ you can see it through my field-glasses,” and, for a joke he pulled a hair from his head and put it across the glass. The lady was delighted, and said, ‘ Oh, yes, I can see the equator distinctly, and there is a camel walking across it.’ Stories of domestic infelicity are very rarely selected by English children, whereas, with American children there are many of them. As examples, the following may be quoted : (a) A wife hearing her husband, in his sleep, murmuring the word Irene, asked him in the morning for an explanation. He told her that it was the name of a horse he had backed at the races. When he returned home at night she said, ‘ Oh, by the way, that horse you dreamt about last night has been ringing you up on the telephone.” (b) A mother said to her little boy, ‘ Come, Georgie, and kiss your new governess.” The boy refused ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 139 and on being asked the reason, said, ‘ I'm afraid to do so mother, because when dad kissed her yesterday she slapped his face.” (c¢) A young man, who had fallen in love with a nice girl, wrote to his father and asked him whether he would advise him to propose. In the presence of his wife the father replied, ‘ By all means, settle matters immediately ; married life is much better than single blessedness,” to which he added, ‘ P.S.—Your mother has just left the room ; don’t be a fool. Stop single.” (4) A man dreamt that another man was running away with his wife, and that he had caught him. ‘ Well,’ said his wife, ‘ what did you say to him.” To which he replied, ‘ I asked him what he was running for.’ Class-room incidents form the basis of far more stories of American than of English children under 12 years of age, but with older children the positions are reversed. The child delights when slips are made by those in authority as when a teacher said, ‘ Johnny, you must never say ain’t, there ain’t no such word in the dictionary.” He is also interested in disputes of teachers with parents: a teacher wrote to a parent and said ‘ There is an odour coming from your son and I think a bath would do him good.” To this the mother replied, ‘ My son is not a rose, don’t smell him, just teach him.” The child also likes to argue with the teacher, a story of which the following is fairly typical: ‘ You're a very naughty boy,” said the teacher, ‘ and I should like to see your father.” ‘ You can’t do that,’ replied the boy. ‘He's a doctor and charges five dollars a visit.” ‘ Then I shall write to him,” she retorted, but the boy persuaded her not to do this on the ground that ‘ it would make mother so jealous.’ The home is also interested in class-room experiences : A mother inquired of her son whether he had obtained any marks at school that day, to which the boy replied that he had got several, but they were where they did not show. Similarly, a father, to stimulate his son to greater 140 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER industry, called his attention to the fact that whereas his clever sister had obtained many prizes and certificates the brother had never had a single certificate. ‘ Yes I have,’ he said, ‘ I got one for being born.’ At 10 years of age English children, as we have seen, become critical and make fun of inspectors and teachers. Some stories of the American children indicate a similar attitude to those in authority : (a) An inspector, in examining a class in history, asked the children to give him a description of a pilgrim, and received the answer, ‘ A pilgrim is a man who goes about from place to place.” ‘Well, then,’ said the inspector, ‘I also go about from place to place, so I suppose that I am a pilgrim.” ‘Oh, no, sir,” said the child. ‘A pilgrim is a good man.’ (6) The boys at a school were given a sum for homework : ‘ How long will it take a man walking at the rate of three and a half miles per hour to go round a field, of given dimensions, two and a half times.’ One of the boys telephoned through to the school on the following morning asking to be excused attendance as ‘ father is doing the walking and I am acting as time-keeper.’ (c) In some schools the meaning of certain words is explained by the teacher and then, in order to see whether the meaning has been properly grasped, the children have to construct sentences containing the words. The teacher had explained that the word excavate meant ‘ to hollow out’. One of the sentences sent up ran as follows : ‘ John hit me and I excavated ’. ; It has been shown that the distinguishing feature, in comparing English and American humour, as seen by the selection of funny stories by the children, is the great prominence given by American children to those based on exaggeration and gross absurdity. A few examples of each will be sufficient. In some of the exaggeration stories there are indications of the Mark Twain influence. ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 141 (a) A fisherman, having caught an enormous fish, found that it was impossible to land it. He therefore tied the line to a tree on an island and the fish was so powerful that it pulled the island a mile to the north-east. (3) An Arizona farmer told an audience that it was so hot in his country that he had to give ice to his hens with their food in order to prevent them laying hard-boiled egg. (c) An English boy told an American boy that in England the houses were so high that they could hardly see the top of them. °That’s nothing,” said the other, ‘ in America they are so high that we have to take them down at night to let the moon pass by.” (d) Pat said that in the war he had one arm blown off. Later on he caught two Germans and knocked their heads together. He was reminded that he had lost one arm. ‘Yes, he replied, ‘but in my excitement I forgot all about it.’ The gross absurdity stories fall naturally into those of a domestic character and those of general interest. In the first division are the following: (a) Mr. Jones and Miss Smith were engaged, and each wanted to give the other a suitable Christmas present. Mr. Jones sold his watch to buy haircombs and Miss Smith had her hair cut off and sold it to buy a watch chain. (b) A child asked her mother how she came to get married, to which she replied, ‘ some one rang me up on the telephone and asked me to marry him and I consented ; he had rung up the wrong number.’ (¢) A nervous young man asked a pretty girl if he might kiss her. ‘First of all,” she said, ‘ I want to know if you have ever kissed a girl before.” ‘I cannot tell you a lie, he said, ‘I have.’ ‘All right then by all means,” she replied, ‘ but I wasn’t going to let you practise on me.’ In the second division may be quoted : (2) A woman, in lecturing to a small audience said, ‘ Men are so much more modest than women and they care so much less about dress. Why the handsomest man in this room has forgotten to put a tie on.” There were only nine men present and 142 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER they all felt for their ties. (b) A scout-master returned to the log cabin very late. The door was locked so he got in through the window and saw a notice up, ‘ Have gone fishing, key outside under flat stone.” (c) The prisoner in disgust threw the magazine across the cell. ‘Hard luck,” he said, ‘I have to be executed on Friday and all these stories are to be continued in our next.” (d) A new traveller was told to wire any important news to the firm. The first day, a message was received from him : ‘ Arrived safely, good room with bath, feeling fine.” To which the firm replied, ‘ So glad, love and kisses, good-bye.” (¢) A soldier was being flogged and laughed continuously. The harder the flogging the more he laughed. The Sergeant was puzzled and said, ‘ What are you laughing about ? ‘ You're flogging the wrong man,’ he said. (f) A man who visited America for the first time, in giving his impressions to a friend, said that what struck him most was the almost universal complaint of the shortage of bananas (* Yes! we have no bananas’). It is curious that children, in recording stories of absent- mindedness, very rarely associate this failing with women, and the man is almost invariably referred to as a professor : A brigand in attacking an absent-minded professor used the expression, ‘If you move youre dead.” The professor objected to this loose form of statement and said, ‘ My dear sir, you really ought to be more careful about your English. If I move it shows that I am alive.” Another absent-minded professor, who had retired to rest, felt rather nervous because he thought he heard a noise under the bed. At last he summoned up courage and asked, ‘Is there anyone there.” ‘No, professor,’ answered the intruder. ‘ Well, that’s very strange,” he said, ‘I was almost sure that I heard someone under the bed.’ Similarly, when the story centres around niggardliness, excessive thrift or hard bargaining the story is generally attributed to a Scotsman, or, if further particularized, an * ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 143 Aberdonian. This is as common in America as it is in England : A Scotsman and his wife were anxious to have a ride in an aeroplane, but could not agree upon terms with the owner. Finally it was arranged that every time the Scotsman called out during the trip the charge would be five dollars. At the conclusion of the ride the Scotsman said to the owner, ‘I nearly gave way when Maggie fell out.” A somewhat gruesome story is also told of a Scots- man who, finding his wife had passed away in her sleep, ran to the door and called out, ‘ Mary, cook only one egg for breakfast.’ Far and away the most popular story, and the one most frequently quoted in the investigation, was that of ‘ Tom Sawyer painting the fence.” A very large number of boys and a fair proportion of the girls selected it as the funniest story they had ever read or heard. It also took a very high place among the stories recorded by English children. Other stories are clearly based upon it as, for example : A lady gave a boy fifty cents to clear away the snow from her front door. He got his friends to come and play snow- ball with him and so got rid of the snow. A similar story is told of a boy who was employed to remove a large heap of stones. In this case the boy induced his friends to join in a stone-throwing competition, with the desired result. Many stories are told of unsuccessful fishermen, who, on their way home purchased fish at the fishmongers. In one case the man induced the dealer to throw the purchased fish to him so that he might truthfully tell his wife that he had caught them. Most of these stories, however, turn on the discrepancy between the number referred to by the fisherman and those charged for in the fishmonger’s bill. Girls of 10 and 11 years of age are particularly interested in the smart sayings of young children, and quote them as being favourite stories: An old lady said to a little boy, ‘Don’t you think your mother would let you come and 144 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER live with me if I gave her a thousand dollars ? ° He replied ‘No, I don’t think so, because you see there are five of us and she wouldn't like to break the set.” Another little boy was asked how old he was. He said, ‘I am four when I go by train, five when I go to the movies, and six when I go to school.” And, again, a grandmother found her little grandson smoking a cigarette and said, * Oh, Johnny, what would your mother say if she saw you!’ and he replied, ‘ She’d spank me because they are her cigarettes.’ In the range of material which taps springs of laughter in the American children, it is seen that when the illustra- tions represent similar groups of humorous situations— apart from an age basis—there is to be observed a very considcraple similarity to the stories selected by English children. The American, biased in favour of exaggeration and gross absurdity, can, however, be readily noticed not only in the individual story but in the selection of a far larger proportion of those stories which rely on these characteristics for their appeal. In making an age survey much wider differences make their appearance. This might reasonably be expected on the ground that there is in America a far greater mixture of races than there is in this country. Moreover, of many countries of which a fair proportion of representatives is to be found in such a city as New York, scarcely a single child of the particular nationality may be present in any London school. The normal order of appeal of a special type of laughter- provoking situation from age to age, which may be dis- covered in the population in which foreign elements play no important part, is in this case lacking. The marked differences, which have already been seen in the general statement of the result of the investigation, are, in large measure, unquestionably due to the racial factor. Another factor is that children being, in consequence of their suggestibility, unduly affected in their appreciation of humorous situations by the attitude of other children ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 145 with whom they are associated in the home or, more especially, in the school, tend to accept the verdict of the group as to what is, or is not, an appropriate cause for laughter. These two factors, however, are further compli- cated by the foreign child being brought into contact with a standard of humour in the land of his adoption which may differ considerably from that of his native country. It is thus evident that an age survey, while far more interesting from a psychological point of view, is very difficult in the presence of so many conflicting elements. It must be almost as embarrassing to the child to change his attitude to subjects producing laughter as to that of a new language. In selecting from the very large number of stories contributed by the children in the different age- groups, those will be taken as examples which are fairly typical of the particular age, largely irrespective of their value from the humorous standpoint. However careful one may be in the choice of such examples, it will be practically impossible to conform to any definite rules for their selection, but the most obvious course must be to regard the number of stories of the particular type recorded by the children as the controlling factor. Here and there we find a story prevalent among, say, the 14-year-old children quoted by the 8-year-old child and vice versa. This is an uncommon experience when the children are practically all of the same nationality. Children of 8 and g years of age quote a large number of fairy, animal, and domestic stories. As in England, the story of the Three Bears is very popular, as it contains fairy, animal, and domestic elements and thus makes a triple appeal to the child. Alice in Wonderland is frequently referred to. One boy expresses his admiration for this story, by saying, ‘ I like Alice in Wonderland because words come in when you don’t expect them. It is very thoughtfully written.” A girl, in a Christmas story, came to the conclusion that Santa Claus was very wicked 10 146 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER because he used such bad language when he fell over a chair in her bedroom on Christmas Eve. At these ages children are very fond of playing with words. A few examples of this are the following : (a) ‘ A baby is like a Ford, it has a new rattle every day.” (b) ‘ A Chinaman is like an author because his tail comes out of his head.” (c) A child, on seeing mummies in the museum for the first time said, ‘I am very glad my mummie isn’t like that.” (d) A tramp having refused to visit a certain house again, his fellow- tramp accused him of being afraid of the dog. ‘No,’ he said, ‘ but my trousers are frayed on account of the dog.’ (¢) ‘ The more you lick a horse the faster it goes. It’s the same with candy.’ Among the domestic stories recorded at these ages the following are fairly typical : (4) A man was unconscious after a serious accident, and the doctor who was called in, after a hurried examination, told his wife that he was dead. The man was just recovering consciousness when he heard the verdict. After the doctor had gone he said to his wife, ‘I'm not dead,’ and she replied, ‘ Oh, John, you must be mistaken, the doctor knows best.” (4) A boy asked his father whether he could write with his eyes shut, and he replied that he could do it quite easily. The boy was much relieved and asked him to sign his report-card with his eyes shut. (c) A grandmother explained to her little grandson that the reason why she wore spectacles was because it made things look so much bigger. Whereupon the boy said, ‘ Granny, will you please take off your glasses before you cut me my piece of cake?’ (4d) A childs father refused to give him a quarter because he was broke. The following day the boy said, ‘ Dad, are you fixed yet ? I want that quarter.” (¢) A father was rebuking his son for inattention and said, ‘ Everything I say to you goes in at one ear and comes out at the other.” ‘Is that the reason why we have two ears, dad ? ’ inquired the boy. (f) A maidservant, scolded by her mistress for not watering ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 147 the rubber plants, excused herself by saying that she thought they were waterproof. Unlike the English, the American children at this stage tell many stories of class-room incidents, and have evidently already acquired the critical attitude : (a) A teacher was lecturing to her children on monkeys, but one boy was paying no attention and was looking out of the window. ‘ Look at me, Johnny,’ she said, ‘ or you won't know what a monkey is like.” (bd) A boy, having been given, as home- work, a long bill of groceries to do, went to a store and asked the manager how much he would have to pay if he bought the various items at the prices stated. The store- keeper worked it out and showed him the bill. The boy, to his surprise, took the bill, thanked him for his kindness and left the store. (c¢) They were conjugating the verb ‘ to scream’ in school, and one boy said, ‘I scream, you scream, we all scream for ice-cream.” (d) They were having a history lesson, and the teacher asked a boy what Sir Walter Raleigh said to Queen Elizabeth when he put down his cloak for her. His reply was, ‘ Step on it, kid.’ There are many more domestic stories quoted by American than by English children. One of the most common is what is known as the ‘gazinta’ story. At school they had been doing arithmetic, and the form of instruction had been: 2 goes into 4 twice; 3 goes into 9 three times; 5 goes into 20 four times, and so on. A boy asked by his mother what subject he liked best at school, replied, ‘ Gazinta’. It was some time before she realized that this was the boy’s interpretation of ‘ goes into’. The popularity of this story is not confined to children in the American schools. The fact that it appeals to the element of superiority (stupidity of the boy) and also includes a play upon words is probably the cause. At these ages more than fifteen per cent of the stories are common to both countries. Children of 10 years of age. At this period there are ! 148 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER many changes. There are fewer fairy and domestic stories. Occasional stories from good literature, such as those from Alice in Wonderland, are told by children of g years of age ; but none of these are now recorded. There are also fewer of the old stories related than at the earlier stage. Stories of great extravagance are now becoming common. Those concerned with the play upon words occur more frequently, and a few conundrums are reported. The girls’ stories at this age are much better than those of the boys’, and a few give some slight indication of subtlety. There is also some increase in the proportion of class-room stories and the critical attitude is becoming more firmly established. Among the stories of great extravagance, selected by the boys, are to be found : (4) A man bought his wife a hat, and ran all the way home with it fearing that it would be out of fashion before he got there. (b) A little girl who was in the upper berth of a sleeping car kept asking her mother and father whether they were there—in the lower berth. An irate passenger shouted, ‘ Yes, we are all here, go to sleep.” The child, after a pause, said in a low voice, ‘ Mother, was that God talking ? ° (c¢) The Governor of the State addressing prisoners at a penitentiary said, ‘ Fellow citizens,” and then seeing his mistake, in his embarrassment, said, ‘ Halloa, boys, glad to see so many of you here to-night.” (d) A Scotsman, in telling a story, said, ‘ In an old castle in Scotland I found some wires laid which proved that my countrymen, in olden times, knew all about the telephone.” Whereupon, an Irishman said, ‘I found no wires in an old castle I visited in Ireland, which proved that the Irish, in olden times, knew all about wireless.” (¢) Three men were debating about ‘The Closest Race’. The first said, ‘A yacht was newly painted and won the race by the thickness of the coat of paint.” The second said, ‘ There was a horse race, and a bee stung one of the horses on the nose and he won by ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 149 the swelling.” The third said, ‘I am quite certain that the closest race is the Scotch.’ The extravagant stories selected by the girls at this age are of a very different order : (a) A man’s store was raided by monkeys, who stole a number of hats and wore them. The man, who was wearing a hat of the same kind, took it off and threw it into the street. The monkeys imitated him, and so he regained his lost property. (b) A mouse’s tail was bitten off by a cat, and he tried to induce the other mice to have their tails cut off, so that he might not look peculiar, but they were too wise. (¢) A mother was much concerned at finding a lot of holes in Tommy’s pants. The boy’s explanation was that he and his friends had been playing at ‘groceries’, and that, unfortunately, he had been cast for the part of ‘ the Swiss cheese’. (d) A boy was found standing up on his bed, and he gave as the reason that he had dreamt that his brother was playing ‘ The Star Spangled Banner’. In the class-room stories, although the number is increasing, there are few of any special interest. The earlier development of the critical attitude spreads this type over a wider range of age, whereas in England it is concentrated in this special period, and the stories there- fore reach a much higher level. Those of the American children are generally associated with the mistakes made in class: (a) Asked to give the name of an extinct bird, a small boy replied, * My canary, he was extincted by our cat.’ (b) The subject of education was being discussed in the home, and a visitor asked one of the boys, * Where do you stand in school?’ To which came the unexpected response, ‘Generally in the corner.’ (c) A teacher impressed upon her children, in a health talk, that when- ever they sneezed they should clap their hands. Soon afterwards a boy, who had fallen into a well, was being hauled up by a rope to which he was clinging. Just as he was approaching the top he sneezed, with the inevitable 150 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER result. (There are many variants of this story). (d) A teacher included under common nouns the word pants (trousers). A boy objected to this, as he thought it was a very uncommon noun because pants were singular at the top and plural at the bottom. Stories based on the play upon words are gradually improving in quality and are extending their range, though naturally they are still of a somewhat primitive type : (a) A boy was seen to be sobbing at a party, when one of the guests was singing a song called, * My old Kentucky Home.’ ‘ Why are you crying, my dear ? ’ inquired an old lady. ‘Are you a Kentuckian?’ ‘No,’ he replied, ‘I am a musician.” (b) A girl was thanking her fiancé for some flowers he had brought, and said, ‘ They are lovely, and so fresh that there is some dew on them yet.” Yes, but I will pay it to-morrow,” reported the embarrassed young man. (c¢) ‘No,’ said the humorist, ‘ Uncle Tom's Cabin was not written by a female hand but by Mrs. Beecher Stowe ’ (Beecher’s toe). (d) The advertisement, * Wanted a single man who can milk and drive a Ford car,’ is fre- quently referred to by the children. (¢) A man, wanting to get information about a boy who had narrowly escaped drowning, asked him, ‘ How did you come to fall in the river?’ ‘I didn’t,’ he replied. ‘I came to fish and then I fell in.” (f) Tommy had been playing truant and had gone fishing. A friend asked him on his return if he had caught anything, and he said, ‘ No, I haven’t been home et.’ Chaldren of 11 years of age. One of the most important differences at this age is the increase of stories of an extra- vagant nature including some of Irish stupidity. In this connexion the girls’ are now quite as extravagant as the boys’ stories. The fact that this age embraces the beginning of the period of rapid growth is partly respon- sible for this well-marked change. Another special feature is a renewed interest in stories of young children, some of ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 151 which indicate remarkable childish precocity. There is also further evidence in some of the stories of the gradual appreciation of the element of subtlety. A few examples are given of extravagant stories recorded by the boys : (a) Pat, in giving an account of his adventures in the war, related how a bullet went into his chest and came out of his back. He was reminded that in this case it might have gone through his heart and have killed him. ‘Oh, no,” he said, ‘that would have been impossible because my heart was in my mouth.” (5) A man described an adventure in which he was terrified because a huge lizard ran up his arm. ‘ Oh, that is nothing,’ said a friend, ‘ one day a sewing machine ran up the seam of my trousers.’ (¢) A nervous tramp asked a lady, at whose house he was calling, ‘ Does your dog bite ?° Yes,” she said, ‘ he does ; but you must not come in because we are very particular about what he eats.” (d) Two men were telling yarns, and one said, ‘ Do you know the story of the guide who showed the people at a museum two skulls of Cleopatra, one as a girl and the other as a woman ?’ ‘ No,’ said his friend, ‘ tell me all about it.” (¢) An old lady asked the officer in charge of the line whether it would kill her if she put her foot on the rail. ‘Yes, madam,” he replied, ‘it would if you put the other on the overhead trolley wire.’ The following examples of extravagant stories selected by the girls are somewhat abnormal. It is possible that, as has been suggested, the girls enter the rapid growth stage rather earlier than the boys, and this is partially confirmed by the increased favour shown, at the age of 11, for this type of story: (a) A sportsman, having run short of ordinary ammunition shot at a stag, using cherry pips instead of the usual material. It was only wounded. Years after he saw the stag again, with two cherry trees growing out of its head. He killed it and had venison and cherry sauce for dinner. (5) A man received a warning : ‘ If you don’t give me two thousand dollars I will kidnap 152 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER your wife.” The man replied, ‘I haven't two thousand dollars, but your proposition interests me greatly.” (c) A man made a bet that he would make his parrot say ‘ Hello’ as if he were at the telephone. After trying for some time, the sleepy parrot opened one eye and said, ‘Line's busy.’ As evidence of the revived interest, at this age, in the affairs of small children the following examples may be quoted : (a) A child at the Zoo said, ‘ Oh, mother, isn’t that gorilla like uncle?’ ‘ Hush, you mustn't say such things,” said the mother. ‘But, mother, dear,” retorted the child, ‘ the gorilla cannot understand me.” (b) A boy, going on an errand, was told by his mother to see if the butcher had pig’s feet. On his return, he said, ‘ I couldn’t find out, mother, whether he had or not, because he had such long trousers on.” (c) A child, on her first visit to the country, seeing a woman plucking a chicken, asked if it had to be undressed like that every night. (d) A boy gives an original account of a domestic incident: ‘Our white cat made great friends with a rat, but one day she felt very hungry and ate the rat and that broke up their friendship.’ (e) A little girl was allowed to go to a wedding, and when she spoke to the bride she asked her whether she felt very tired. ‘ No,” said the bride, ‘but why do you ask? “Oh,” replied the child, ‘because I heard mother say that you had been running after Mr. Smith for some years.’ Children of 12 years of age. At this age, which is well within the period of rapid growth, there is a decline in the sense of humour. The stories are to a large extent of a comparatively low order, and consist principally of records of gross absurdity or of exaggeration. The breaking away from authority and tradition is clearly marked. The redeeming feature is the selection of many stories about children, in some of which the child gets the advantage in verbal encounters with the parent. Irish stories of ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 153 stupidity are exceptionally popular. On the whole the girls’ stories are of a much better type than those of the boys.’ It will be seen that in the boys’ extravagant stories there is considerable variety, which is also characteristic of the period: (a) A girl, describing her fiancé, said that he was a bit of a lawyer, and a bit of a musician, and added that ‘ The lawyers regard him as a musician, and the musicians are of the opinion that he is a lawyer.’ (b) Mike, walking through a forest, went to catch a parrot who greeted him with, ‘ Hello, Mike.” ‘I beg yer pardon,’ said Mike, ‘ I thought you was a bird.” (¢) A man went to a charitable lady and drew a harrowing picture of a poor family who were in danger of being turned into the streets unless £10 could be collected for the rent. ‘ Who are you,” said the lady. The man, brushing away his tears, replied, ‘ I'm the landlord.” (4) A man called for his boots which he had left to be repaired by the bootmaker, who would, however, only give him one boot as he had become a bankrupt, and was only paying ten shillings in the pound. (¢) A dog committed suicide by turning round and round and at last, on catching his tail, he exclaimed, ‘ This is the end of me.” (f) A woman went into a drug store and said, ‘I want some canine pills.” ‘ What is the matter with the dog,’ inquired the manager. The woman replied indignantly, * My husband isn’t a dog.’ Whereupon he gave her some quinine pills. The girls’ stories in this category reach a much higher level than those of the boys’ and are far more interesting : (2) In a crowded train a man, wanting a seat, sang, ‘ The Star Spangled Banner’. All the passengers stood up, and then he was able to secure one. (b) A policeman asked a girl to marry him. She refused, and then he took her into custody for ‘resisting a police officer’. (c) A weeping woman said that the reason why she was overcome by grief was that she had no money to pay for the christening 154 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER of her baby. A man gave her a five-dollar bill, and told her to bring him the change (two dollars). The result was general happiness. The mother was happy because her baby was christened ; the priest was happy because he received his fee of three dollars; and the man was happy because he had got rid of a bad five-dollar note. (d) A post was advertised at an institution, and a boy thought it might suit him and so he made inquiries. The secretary in enumerating the necessary qualifications said, ‘ He must be a gentleman, keep himself clean, brush his hair every day, be a non-smoker, never swear nor drink, and Here he was interrupted by the applicant, who said, ‘ You don’t want a boy, you want a girl for this job.’ (¢) A painter in an asylum kept looking at a plain canvas, and told an inquiring visitor that it was his great master- piece of ‘ The passage of the Israelites over the Red Sea.’ The visitor said he could not see the sea, the Israelites nor the Egyptians. ‘Of course not,” said the painter, ‘ the sea has been driven back, the Israelites have crossed over, and the Egyptians haven't arrived yet. (f) A lecturer on Ireland was explaining why green was the Irish colour. He said that there was once a great plague of monkeys in Ireland, and a body of men went to drive them out. In order to tell them apart from the monkeys each man had to wear a piece of green ribbon ; and after that green was regarded as the national colour. The stories about children are becoming increasingly interesting. It is difficult at first to account for the great popularity of the following : A mother alligator said to a baby alligator, ¢ What will you become when you're grown up,” and the baby alligator replied, ‘ A travelling bag.’ But an animal story, with the added interest given by the fairy element in the power of speech, together with a quaint reply to the mother’s question, provides sufficient lines of appeal to give such a story a certain survival value. Its popularity, moreover, is not confined to ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 155 children of this particular age. The enthusiasm for stories which survive far beyond the age at which they are first selected by the children may frequently be traced to the possession by them of more than one element of appeal. It has already been pointed out that there is a certain breaking away from authority at this age, and that the critical attitude is rampant. In these circumstances criticism of, and love of argument with, parents or others in authority would naturally be dear to the child’s heart. They find expression in such stories as : (a) * Mother, why do brides always dress in white ? ’ inquired a small boy. To which the mother replied, ‘ Because white denotes joy.” ‘Then why do the bridegrooms wear black ? ’ said the boy. The only retort was, ‘ Go on with your break- fast.” (b) The train was crowded, and a mother asked her boy what he would do if he saw a lady standing up. He said, ‘I should do what father always does, I should pretend to be asleep.” (c) A mother was talking to her children about the sin of naughtiness. * If you're naughty,’ she said, ‘ remember, your children will be naughty too.’ ‘ Oh, mother,” exclaimed one of the boys, ‘ what a naughty girl you must have been.’ In other stories about children there is a certain amount of playful criticism of home affairs such as the desire to please everybody as in the statement of the child : ‘ When I grow up I shall be a minister to please mother, and then a lawyer to please father, and then I shall become a police- man to please myself.” It is very difficult in some cases to tell whether children are serious or stupid or simply making fun of those in authority in the recorded stories. A mother noticed that her boy was washing himself far beyond the accustomed time for the operation, and inquired the reason, to which he replied, ‘ I am washing myself seven times so that I need not wash again for a week.” The reason for the selection of such a story as a favourite was probably because it ministered to the feeling 156 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER of superiority of the selector, who simply regarded it as a | good story of a stupid boy. Children of 13 years of age. There is a marked improve- ment at this age in the type of story selected by the chil- dren. This is particularly noteworthy in the case of the stories chosen by the girls, which are generally of a different | and more refined quality than those of the boys. The period of rapid growth appears to affect the sense of | humour of the boys more than it does that of the girls, who seem to recover from the set-back more readily. The stories of absent-mindedness are slightly on the increase, | but they still constitute a very small proportion of the | whole. Stories of Irish :tupidity are still popular but | markedly less so than with the twelve year old children. There are still some excellent stories about young children. The kind of extravagance referred to in the boys’ stories | is rather less associated with gross absurdity than in those previously quoted as being selected by younger children : | (a) Some guinea-pigs were sent by rail, and the authorities had no obvious category in which to place them. They were clearly not chargeable in such divisions of animals | as, for example, pigs, mice, canaries, or squirrels. The | matter was referred to an eminent zoologist, who wrote a | long memorandum on the subject without coming to a definite conclusion. In the meantime the guinea pigs were increasing in number so rapidly that the railway company let them through free of charge. (6) A man sat in the electric chair, and the sympathetic governor of the | prison told him that if he had any last wish it should be granted to him. Without hesitation, he said, ‘I should like to give up my seat to a lady.” (c¢) Three men were singing the praises of the countries which they represented— England, Scotland, and Ireland. At the end of the discussion the Englishman said, ‘ If I were not an English- man I'd like to be a Scotsman.” Whereupon the Scotsman oN ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 157 said, ‘ If I were not a Scotsman I'd like to be an English- man.” But the Irishman said, ‘ If I were not an Irishman I should be ashamed of myself.” (4) A woman in a drapery store asked a man, by mistake, to show her some silk things. When he brought them she said, ‘ Oh, I must be mad, I want muslin.” An attendant overhearing this, ran out for a policeman, and said, ‘ Come at once. There's a mad woman in the store who wants muzzling.’ The girls’ stories of extravagance are of a very different order : (a) A clergyman was delighted at seeing a confirmed drunkard at the evening service, and told him the next day how glad he was to have seen him in church. ‘At church! Oh, that’s where I was last night,” he observed. (6) A soldier had a bullet-proof shirt made for him. Later on, his wife found several holes in it and told her husband. ‘ Yes,” he said, ‘I sent it to the laundry.” (¢) A man at the back of a hall at a lecture kept on shouting out ‘Louder!’ At last a man in the front row stood up and said, ‘ Can’t you hear what the lecturer is saying ? ° ‘ No,’ said the man. ‘ Well,” he retorted, ‘ we can, and you ought to be very thankful, sit down.” (d) A careful wife said to her husband, ‘ John, that suit of yours is very shabby. It looks as if you had been sleeping in it.” ‘I wore it last Sunday at church,” he replied. In the stories about children at this age, there appears to be a partial reversion to accounts of mistakes made by them as the basis rather than of verbal encounters between the child and parent or teacher. Of those selected by the boys the following are fairly typical: (¢) A boy con- gratulates his father on having saved five dollars, because he had promised him that sum if he were promoted to a higher class at the end of the term, whereas he had been left behind in the same class. (b) A discussion was taking place between two boys about the division of an apple, the older boy having kept the larger part. The younger said that if he had divided it he would have given the 158 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER other the larger part. ‘ Well, then,” said he, ‘ what is there to grumble about ? In any case you would have had the smaller part.” (c) A plumber went to a house and said to the little boy at the door, ‘ I’ve come to fix that old tub in the kitchen.” The boy shouted out, ‘ Mother, the doctor has called to see cook.” (d) The teacher wanted to help the children to remember to say, ‘ It is 1’, instead of ‘It is me’. She therefore taught them to say, ‘It is I, said the spider to the fly.” One of the boys, however, made the same mistake the following day, and she said, ‘ Tommy, what did I teach you yesterday ?° To which he replied, ‘ It is me, said the spider to the flea.’ In the girls’ stories about the doings of children there is considerable variety: (a) Some fathers were talking about the excellent qualities of their children. After hearing many opinions expressed, one of them said, * Now I have a perfect son, he never smokes, doesn’t go to clubs, and never comes home late.” They all said, ‘ What a splendid fellow!’ Then, one inquired, ‘ How old is he ? The father replied, ‘ Six months.” (4) A little boy went to a farmhouse, and was interested to see the eggs brought in. ‘Do cows lay eggs?’ he asked. His sister, who was proud of her knowledge of country life, said, ‘ Of course not, you stupid boy, how could a cow get into the chicken coop.” (c¢) A policeman found a boy crying on the side-walk, and asked him the reason. ‘ Mother said I wasn’t to cross the street before all the automobiles have passed, and none have passed yet,” he said. (4) ‘ Daniel, what a dirty hand you have,” said the teacher. ‘If you can find another hand as dirty as that I won’t punish you.’ Daniel produced his other hand from behind, and he was let off. The number of girls’ stories of stupidity at this age are considerably in excess of those of the boys. Children of 14 years of age. Great changes are to be observed at this age in the nature of the stories selected. The rapid growth period, with its demoralizing effect upon "ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 159 the appreciation of humour, has passed Throughout, there is a very marked improvement, especially in the boys’ selections. There are still many ext avagant stories but now these generally possess other elements than exaggeration or gross absurdity to justify them. The girls still quote more stupidity stories than the boys. Stories about children have practically disappeared, and many of the domestic stories are obtained from good literature. Absent-mindedness is, still, occasionally the cause of laughter. Frequently, traces of subtlety have a beneficent effect upon the stories. Among the extravagant stories there is a much wider range and also a greater variety : (2) An Englishman was enjoying lunch with a Chinaman, and thought he was eating duck in the stew provided. To mark his enjoyment he rolled his eyes, stroked his stomach, and said, Quack, quack.” The Chinaman shook his head and said, ‘ Bow wow.” (b) A traveller going up hill in a bus could not understand why the conductor kept opening and shutting the door. He explained that it was done to encourage the horses, who thought from the noise that passengers were getting out of the bus. (c¢) An officer told his men to fight until all their ammunition was gone and then to run for their lives. ‘I am rather lame,” he said, ‘so I think I'll start now.” (d) A tramp gave a charitable lady an abnormally long list of diseases from which he was suffer- ing. She suggested that it would be more effective if he concentrated on one or two.- ‘No, kind lady,” said the tramp. ° There is nothing so fatal to the full development of one’s natural powers as narrow specialization.” (¢) In a cinema production, a nervous man had to go into a lion’s den. The director said, ‘ Don’t be afraid, that lion was brought up on milk.” ‘Yes! so was I,” he said, ‘ but I eat meat now.’ The stories of absent-mindedness are still associated with professors, such, for example, as: A professor was 160 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER told by a burglar to lie still while he was looking for money. ‘ Looking for money,” said the professor, ‘ do let me come with you.” Another professor was lecturing to some friends and said, ‘I will now show you the various parts of a frog which I have brought with me.” He opened the packet and found there some sandwiches and cake. ‘ That's strange,’ he said, ‘ I felt certain that I had eaten my lunch.’ Among the few stories recorded, at this age, about children are: A gentleman seeing a little boy trying to ring a bell, rang it for him. Whereupon, the boy said, ‘ I think we had better run now.” (0) A child was told to draw a horse and wagon but only drew a horse. Asked the reason, he said, ‘I thought the horse would draw the wagon.” (¢) A mother suggested to her little son that he should give a poor boy his bunny because he had no father. The boy didn’t like losing his rabbit, and said, ‘ Mother, wouldn't it do as well if we gave him father ?’ Children of 15 and 16 years of age. The improvement obse ved at the age of fourteen is continued, and is fairly equally shared between boys and girls. The tendency to select stories based on good literature is further developed and is not, as at the lower ages, so much confined to the writings of the great triumvirate of humorists—the Mark Twain school. Reference has already been made to the revival of an interest in playing with words at this stage, and this is common to the stories about adults and also those intimately concerned with the doings of children. The pun has never become popular in America, but occasionally in this revival it may be the turning point of the story. Stories of absurdity, especially at the age of fifteen represent a much smaller proportion and, as a rule, they are not entirely dependent upon the element of extravagance lor their selection. Here and there, we still find stories based on absent-mindedness. As examples of absurdity stories we may mention the following : (a) A motorist ran over a dog in the road and ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 161 gave the owner, who was carrying a gun, five dollars as compensation. ‘I fear I have interfered with your day’s sport,” said the motorist. ‘Sport,’ exclaimed the man, ‘I was only taking the dog out to that wood to shoot it.’ (This is a member of a large family of stories of this type.) (6) A country cousin went to the theatre for the first time, and saw the play of ‘ Hamlet’. ‘It is a very good play,’ he observed, ‘ but it is a pity that it contains so many familiar quotations.” (c) A girl, getting tired of waiting so long for the wedding day, suggested to her fiancé that they should elope, and get married at once. Just as they were preparing to leave the house, he whispered, ‘ What is that noise, dear?’ ‘Oh,’ she said, ‘it is only father bringing down my trunk.” (4) An absent-minded man saw his own notice on the door, ‘ I shall be back at 3 o’clock.’ He sat down and waited, as he had returned earlier than he had anticipated. (¢) A boy was about to make a remark at dinner, when his stern father said, ‘ You mustn’t talk at meals, my boy. No! not a word.” Afterwards he said, ‘ Now, then, what was it you wanted to say?’ ‘It doesn’t matter now, father,” he said. ‘There was a green worm on your salad but you have eaten it.” (f) A town child, not wishing to be thought ignorant of country life, on seeing honey on the table, said, ‘Ah! I see you keep a bee.” (g) A child, seeing the picture of a man with a halo, said, ‘ Look how that man is carrying his spare tyre’ There is considerable diversity in the stories of this period of those dealing with play upon words: A small girl, seeing her mother waving her hair, stroked her father’s bald head, and said, ‘ No waves for you, daddy—all beach.’ (b) A royal baby had a severe stomach-ache, so that the Court decided to send for the Minister of the Interior. (c) A strap-hanger in a bus lost his grip, and was hurled into a lady’s lap. She was indignant, and said, ‘ I thought you were a gentleman.” ‘ Well,’ he said, ‘I thought that II 162 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER I was an Irishman, but it now appears that I am a Lap- lander.’ In the record of selected stories it has been possible, by a liberal supply of illustrations, to show the main points of similarity and difference, age by age, of the appreciation of material which is productive of laughter in English and American children. The influence of what we have termed the Mark Twain school of humour can be clearly demonstrated. The undoubted superiority of the American children in the domestic stories, of which no statistical evidence is possible, is undoubtedly due, as we have seen, to the successful efforts which have been made to provide suitable, interesting, and humorous children’s books, graded for particular age groups. The numerous stories which are based upon such books provides sufficient evidence that they are fully appreciated by the children. The descriptions of comical sights which have been seen was the subject of a separate investigation. Ample references have been made to such descriptions in dealing with the sense of humour of English children. For purposes of comparison it will be sufficient to give a few examples of accounts of funny sights recorded by American children at different ages. Some of these descriptions are verbatim reports, others are somewhat reduced in length by the omission of irrelevant material. (a) A boy, 12 years of age. ‘1 was in a restaurant the other day where the waiters were mostly darkies. Every- thing was going on peacefully. One of the patrons asked one of the darkies to bring him a platter of turkey with plenty of gravy. The waiter said he would if he would wait a moment. Presently the darky was seen emerging from the kitchen with a delicious platter of roast turkey, with lots of gravy, cranberry sauce and dressings. He was about a yard away from the table of the man who had ordered the turkey, when suddenly he tripped and fell. ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 163 When he had fallen, by some trick or other the plate managed to be under him and the turkey was in his hand. When the guests had stopped laughing, he said sadly, ‘ Lord 0’ Massy fo’ drefful things am happened.” ‘ What,’ I inquired. He answered, ‘ The downfall of Turkey, the spilling of Greece, the breaking of China, and the misery of Africa.’ (6) 4 girl, 12 years of age. ‘One day I was walking with my friend on Fifth Avenue, when I saw such a sight that made my eyes water from laughing. We saw two men walking side by side, one was about six feet tall and the other one was about three feet. There was a crowd going after them, so we heard that the tall man was the son of the short man. They were having a quarrel. The son lifted up the father and talked to him, when the father struck him in the face. That is the funniest thing I ever witnessed.’ (c) A boy, 13 years of age. ‘One day while I was riding in the subway I saw a most singular sight. There was one man who seemed to be very affectionate to a lady who must have been his wife. When they came in he had his arm around her and when they sat down his arm was around her. When the train reached his station, he got, up, but she was up ahead of him. He put his arm around” her, or rather the lady whom he thought her. Imagine his embarrassment when he saw that he had put his arm around an old negress.’ (d) A girl, 13 years of age. ‘On “ Safety Day” my mother and I went to see the parade which was held on Fifth Avenue. One of the funniest things which 1 ever saw was the old-fashioned way of travelling. This amused me, and also interested me very much. To think how the world has changed from 1769 to 1925. The different type of automobiles, wagons, cars, locomotives, coaches, and bicycles. Everybody was very happy. The people, who ~ were sitting in the coaches, were all dressed up just like 12 164 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER in the olden times. How I wish I could see this parade over again. I am very sorry for the girls and boys who have not seen this parade, for it was interesting and would teach us many things in history, and also very amusing.’ (e) A boy, 14 years of age. ‘ A few weeks ago I saw some- thing funny. It may not strike you as humorous but it certainly did so to me. A small dog was walking along the avenue. It was not the kind of dog that you or I might consider well cared for. He had a very long snout and was as dirty as he could be, but nevertheless he had an intelligent look. Just then, a large car came out of the maze on the street and pulled up with a quick stop near this dog. The coloured chauffeur jumped out and opened the door of the limousine. The dog was taking all these actions in, with a cocked head, and was slowly walking toward the car, when a well-dressed man with a high hat stepped out. The man turned his back to the dog without noticing him and started to give directions to his chauffeur. The dog came up and sniffed the man’s shoes and cane. The man suddenly became frantic, and jumped away when he saw the dog at his heels. In the excitement his high hat fell off. At that the dog stopped, and started playing and ripping the hat to pieces. The man jumped back into his car, and was off. But the dog was still playing with the hat.’ (f) A girl, 14 years of age. ‘Once, while I was coming home, a woman came out of a shoe store. Her old shoes she was carrying in her arm, and her new shoes she was wearing. As she was walking out of the shoe-store her heel caught in the car-track. This was her first pair of shoes with high heels, so she did not want them to be run over by a car. She tried every way to take out the shoe but she did not succeed. A car was coming along, and she did not know what to do. Then she suddenly remem- bered that she had a red petticoat on. She took off her petticoat and held it up for danger. The people laughed, ENGLISH AND AMERICAN HUMOUR 165 and the conductor stopped the car and helped her get her shoe out.’ (g) A boy, 15 years of age. ‘A Chinaman came to this country about a year ago and settled down to live in Chinatown. He was a jolly fellow and made friends easily. He loved best to walk through the city streets and see the sights. One day, during his rambling, he managed to save a man from being run over. He had been approach- ing Fourteenth Street when he saw a man dart in front of a passing vehicle in his effort to cross the street. He immediately dashed forward and snatched the man from certain death. At first the man was highly indignant at this outrageous treatment, but he became profusely thankful when he learnt the truth from onlookers. There developed a warm friendship between the white and the yellow man. When the white man’s birthday came, the Chinaman presented him with a miniature coffin. In China this is the highest honour that can be paid to anyone. At this, the white man’s face became a study. Doubt, bewilderment, suspicion, and anger struggled for supremacy. When he could control his voice he asked the Chinaman for an explanation. The Chinaman, who had been surprised by his friend’s actions, explained that according to Chinese customs, this was the highest honour that could be paid to anyone.’ : (h) A girl, 15 years of age. ‘One day, while walking with a couple of girl friends, we stopped at a place where many people had gathered. Thinking something very serious had happened, we also stopped to see what was going on. There we saw a little boy sobbing bitterly, while he was being questioned by the policeman. ‘ What is the matter, little boy ? ’ asked the policeman ; but he would only cry, and scratch his back. Then the policeman repeated what he had said, and finally the child said, ‘I was walking down here with my tame bird on my shoulder, and now he has flown away.” The policeman laughed, and 166 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER put his hand down the boy’s back, and out came the little bird. The little boy turned towards the policeman, and said, ‘ Never in my four years did I dream of being a cage.’ I think this little youngster was quite clever, for he made every one around him burst into laughter.’ The greatest difference to be observed in comparing such records of English with those of American children is that, in the former the narrator seems to play a far more important part in making a readable and interesting account of a relatively simple humorous situation. CHAPTER XII THE LAUGHTER OF COLOURED CHILDREN r ! \HE laughter of the coloured child is a matter of pure enjoyment, and retains all the characters of the hearty uncontrolled laughter of the white child in the infants’ department of the school. The spirit of the child, uninfluenced by the disciplinary effect which results in a somewhat modified form of laughter making its appearance as a white child develops, is unduly pro- longed in the act of laughing of the coloured child. It is, in fact, in this as in other respects, the retention of the undiluted spirit of the child in full vigour which marks off quite clearly the great difference in the emotional attitude of the two types of children. The comic sight at which the nine year old white child would smile is greeted with hearty laughter by the coloured child of the same age. It would appear that the coloured child smiles less and laughs much more than the white child. In young children of both types there is a distinct ten- dency to join in the laughter of a group before the cause of merriment has been grasped. This reaction to an environ- ment of hearty laughter is, however, controlled much earlier in the white than in the coloured child. The response of laughter to a humorous situation is much more easily aroused in the latter than in the former variety throughout the whole range of school life. This retention of the unchanged spirit of the child to a much later age is the main cause of the wide divergence of the apprecia- tion of mirth-provoking situations between white and coloured children, as is evidenced by the selection by them of funny stories and jokes, and their descriptions of comical 167 168 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER sights. Stories and, to a less degree, funny sights which have lost their power of appeal to white children, will still cause laughter in the case of coloured children of the same age. Up to the age of twelve years, more than half of the stories of coloured children are selected from those which have their basis in fairy or animal stories, especially if the latter happen to possess a fairy element. The childish outlook thus remains very much longer with coloured than with white children. In mass experiments it is easily possible to discover the kinds of stories which are most popular with the boy or girl of a particular age. Using this as a basis of comparison, it is found that the stories which are selected by coloured children of 12 years of - age are of a similar nature to those selected by the white children at the age of nine. This difference of three years is maintained up to the age of 15 years. If the selection of a favourite story is regarded as a rough indication of the sense of humour reached at a particular stage, the conclusion must naturally be that the sense of humour of the coloured child is three years behind that of the white child. This, however, can only be regarded as an approximate general- ization, as there are other factors which would require investigation before any definite statement could be made. It has been seen that the sense of humour—certainly up to the age of 16 years—of the white girl is as keen; if not keener, than that of the white boy of the same age. With regard to coloured children, however, the boy is considerably ahead of the girl. The play upon words as a source of laughter is retained among coloured children even longer than among American children with whom, as we have noted, it is continued to a later stage than with English children. About twenty per cent of the stories of coloured children are associated with the play upon words. Here and there, also, we find riddles of a some- what primitive order. The element of subtlety is very THE LAUGHTER OF COLOURED CHILDREN 169 rarely found in the stories selected by coloured children. From 12 to 15 years of age they are very keen on Irish stupidity stories. In the stories selected we find that in those of the more extravagant type only comparatively few are influenced, to any considerable extent, by the Mark Twain school. Many of the stories referred to by coloured children are associated with pulpit utterances and other church matters. The favourite story is that of the minister who had been asked by his flock to tell them the meaning of the word ‘ phenomenon ’, which he so frequently used in his sermons. He dealt with it in the following way : ‘ If you see a cow eating grass in a field that is not a phenomenon, and if you hear a bird singing like a canary, up in a tree, that, also, is not a phenomenon ; but if, my friends, you saw a cow sitting up in a tree and singing like a canary, that would be a phenomenon.” The popularity of this story is widespread, and it is frequently quoted. Among other stories dealing with church matters may be mentioned: (4) A minister was somewhat surprised that a certain member of his congregation had been elected a deacon of his church, and he asked him how it came about. ‘Well,’ he said, ‘there’s a lot of disreputable people here and I have been elected to represent them.’ (6) A coloured Baptist preacher was exhorting people to come and have their sins washed away, when a member of his flock said he had had his washed away at the Methodist church. Whereupon, the Pastor said, No, my brother, you ain’t been washed, you've only been dry- cleaned.” (c) A man was brought before the judge on a charge of making whisky, and said, in reply to a question, that his name was Joshua. The judge said, ‘ Are you the Joshua that made the sun stop.” ‘No,’ he replied, ‘I am the man what made de moonshine.” (4) A Minister, remonstrating with a member of his congregation, said, « Surely you're not going to marry again now that the 170 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER Lord has taken your fourth husband.” ‘Sure,’ said the negress, ‘ as long as de Lord takes them, so will I.’ A new type of story is frequently quoted by coloured children, which deals with the black man’s weakness for stealing chickens. Among those selected are: (2) A man was charged with stealing chickens but the evidence was not conclusive, so the judge said, * Sambo, you are acquitted on the charge of stealing chickens for lack of evidence.’ The man did not understand this, and said, ‘ Does that mean, boss, that I shall have to return the chickens?’ ‘No,’ said the judge, ‘guilty; ten days.’ (b) In con- nexion with a charge of chicken-stealing a prisoner declared that the chickens had jumped right into his arms, and he hoped that he might be struck dead in five minutes if what he said was not true. The judge said, ‘ If you are not, you will go to prison for fifteen days.” (c¢) A doctor ordered a sick man to have some chicken soup, and always to go to bed early. ‘ How can I get the soup unless my evenings are free to go out and get the chickens?’ he replied. (4d) A boy was reproved by a judge for again stealing chickens. Turning to the father, he said, ‘ Why don’t you teach your son something?’ ‘I do,’ replied the father, ‘ but he always gets caught.’ As examples of the more extravagant stories chosen by the coloured children are the following: (2) ‘ A bee was on a horse’s tail, and said he couldn’t kill him, and the horse backed on to a tree and bruised himself so badly that he had to go home on crutches, and the bee flew up into a tree and laughed so much that his sides busted.’ (b) Sambo was in love with a negress, but could not sum- mon up sufficient courage to ask her to be his wife, so his master advised him to ring her up on the telephone. He didn’t mention his name but told her all about his worldly possessions, and ended up by saying, ‘ Will you marry me, Miss Lucy?’ ‘Sure I will,’ she replied; ‘what nigger’s this?’ (c) A General asked a coloured man THE LAUGHTER OF COLOURED CHILDREN 171 who wanted to join the army, which he would prefer, the cavalry or the infantry. ‘Infantry,’ he said, ‘ because if there’s any running to be done I don’t want to be bothered with no horse.” (d) A child was baptised Nosmo King Smith, because his mother had been attracted by the names Nosmo and King on the doors of an institution. Afterwards, when the doors were closed, she found that the words simply meant ‘No smoking’. (¢) An old woman was having her pictures cleaned and told the men to be very careful. They suggested that with a little whisky they could be made to look fine. She, therefore, fetched some whisky which they drank when she was not looking and cleaned the pictures with water. She was pleased with the result, and said to the men, ‘I never thought I should have used that whisky again after I had washed my poor dog, Fido, in it when he died.’ The relations of the white masters with their coloured servants are frequently the subject of selected stories ; (a) A great advocate of no slavery entertained a negress at dinner and made a fuss of her. He said, ‘ I guess, Amanda, that your old master never treated you like this.” ‘ Oh no, sir,” she replied, ‘ my master never had me at his table ; he was a gentleman, sir.” (5) A man was telling his coloured servant about his wonderful dog, Rover, who, being locked in the house, got out of a window and followed him for a distance of eight miles, entirely by scent. ‘Oh, massa,” said the boy, ‘I thinks you needed a bath.’ (c) Sambo’s master was very ill, and there was to be a consultation among the doctors after the examination. Anxious to hear what the doctors thought of his case, the master told his servant to hide in the room and let him know what they said. His report was, ‘ They said your heart was fine, and your lungs were fine, and they didn’t know what was the matter with you, but they would find it out all right at the post-mortem.” (d) At the conclusion of a trial a negro was found guilty of a 172 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER serious offence, and the judge informed the prisoner that on the following Monday he would be shot at sunrise. ‘That will be impossible, judge,” he replied. ‘I never get up so early as that.” (¢) A negro asked his employer to let him have a holiday to attend his uncle’s funeral on a certain day in the following month. ‘It is strange,’ he replied, ‘that your uncle should have fixed the date of his own death.” ‘He didn’t, boss, it was the judge,’ was the retort. (There are many variants of this story.) There are continual references to the play upon words in the stories of the coloured children : (2) In announcing the appointment of a coloured boy to the State Legislature, it was stated, ¢ This is not the first dark page in the history of the legislature.” (b) A traveller asked a black conductor whether he liked his quarters, and he replied, ‘ Yas, I likes de quarters heah berry much when I gets ‘’em.” (c) A boy on entering a hospital was asked if he had pyjamas. ‘No,’ said the boy, ‘it’s mumps.’ (d) A negro insisted on speaking of fleas as mathematical fleas. Asked the reason he said, ‘ They subtract from your happiness; divide your attention; add to your misery and multiply rapidly.’ (¢) A father asked his son why he called his sister May’s young man ‘April showers.” ‘Because he always brings May flowers,” was the answer. (f) An Irishman with his mother, wandering in a city, saw many restaurants, and said, There are plenty of places to rest your aunt here, but there are none to rest your mother.’ Among the younger coloured children the most popular stories are those recording stupid answers to a simple question, such as: (a) ‘If a dog stood on a doorstep and howled would that be a sign of death?’ ‘Yes,’ was the answer, ‘ if he stayed there long enough.” (0) An Irishman was asked why he always called his pig Ink. To which he replied, ‘ Because he’s always running out of his pen.’ (c) An old fat woman asked a policeman if he could see her : THE LAUGHTER OF COLOURED CHILDREN 173 across the street. ‘Madam,’ he said, ‘I could see you a mile away.” (4d) A man who said he had never had an accident was asked, ‘ What is the matter with your hand ? ’ ‘ Snake-bite,” he replied. ‘Well, surely that was an accident,” suggested the questioner. ‘No,’ he said, ‘the darned snake did it on purpose.” (¢) A boy was asked in school to give an example of a collective noun. And he replied, ‘A vacuum cleaner.” (f) A coloured boy saved another, with whom he was fishing, from drowning, and was asked why he risked his life for his friend. ‘I was bound to,” he said, ‘ he had all the bait in his pocket.’ A sufficient number of illustrations has been given to indicate the type of story which coloured children appreciate during the period of, approximately, 9 to 15 years of age. It will be noticed that, apart from certain types of humorous situations with which we find no corresponding material in the stories of white children, the general impression is that the attitude shown by these coloured children may be compared roughly with that of English and American children of the age range of 6 to 12 years. It should be remembered, however, that the stories are chiefly from children educated in New York schools, and that records obtained from districts in which the population contains a much larger proportion of coloured children would probably give very different results. It has already been pointed out that it would, however, be dangerous to conclude, from the information available, that there is a difference of three years between the sense of humour of white and coloured children. Some carefully selected records descriptive of funny sights, almost entirely from boys, will show, perhaps most clearly, the attitude of the children towards comical situations. A few of them have been somewhat reduced in length, but the actual description recorded, apart from changes in the spelling of certain words, is in the child’s own language. The age is given in each case. 174 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER (a) (12 years of age). ‘ Once when I was in swimming I got tired and went on a raft. While we were talking on some interesting subject we heard a cry of “sharks”. The cry came from one of the fellows in the water, and all eyes were searching the water for a fin of a shark, but all they could find was a mass of jelly. And when we found out that it was a harmless jelly fish that stung him we laughed until we thought we would bust.’ (0) (x2 years of age). ‘ The funniest sight I saw was as follows : it was snowing and a man was walking down the street and coming towards him were some boys on sleds. All of a sudden they came right up on him and as he went to jump out of the way of one of them he would jump into another one’s way. He managed to escape all until the last boy came full speed right towards him. The man’s arm was full of packages and as the sled struck him, the packages flew up in the air. In one hand he had a dozen eggs and as he fell down he fell right on the top of them. He was a sight for sore eyes when he arose from the ground.’ (¢) (x3 years of age). ‘I was on a train one day and next to me were a little boy and his father. The little boy was playing with his hat and his father thought that he might let go and let it fall. The father wanted to teach the little boy a lesson, so he put his hand out of another window and took the little boy’s hat. The boy was surprised and told his father that he had dropped his hat. The father said, ‘ Don’t worry about that. I will whistle and it will fly in again.’ The father (who had the hat at the back of him) whistled, and threw it in at the window and the son was glad. But the son liked to see him whistle and make the hat come back, so the little boy threw it out (without the father knowing it) and the little boy said to his father, ‘ Papa, whistle.” The man was very sorry that he ever started this.’ THE LAUGHTER OF COLOURED CHILDREN 175 (@) (x3 years of age). ‘ A man who had been a cripple for over fourteen years was taking the fresh air in his wheel chair on the avenue, when a huge truck ran into a coal truck loaded with coal. The other truck containing three wild bulls was broken on the side and the bulls made their way out to the street. The people who had gathered around looking at the accident started to throw up their hands and run, asking God to save them. The cripple, hearing the screams, turned his wheel chair around to see what was happening, when suddenly he saw two of the wild cattle coming straight towards him. This made him scared and excited ; he pulled the blanket off of his legs, jumped out of the wheel chair and up the street he flew, outrunning everybody although he was a cripple before the accident. When everything was over and he was calmed down he came back to his wheel chair. After this he tried to walk several times but failed to do so.’ (e) (x4 years of age). ‘ The funniest sight that I ever saw was when a foreigner, who had just come from the other side, bought an auto- mobile. Thinking that the sidewalks were for automobile purposes, he rode up on the sidewalk. He was surprised when he saw all the people looking at him. Ah,” said he to himself, ““ these people think that I am a Count or a noble from the other side. Ah, the American people are very much refined.” Soon a blue-coated man came along and said, “ Here is a ticket to dine with the judge.” Thinking that the judge was some great personage of the city, he courteously said, “ Tell your master that I am very much honoured.” Then he stood up in his car and said: “ The American people are very kind and generous of heart.” ’ (F) (x4 years of age). - ‘Last summer, while on my uncle’s farm, the roof sprang a leak. Uncle said he would go out and fix it as 176 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER soon as it had stopped raining. When the rain had stopped he took a long ladder, the only one he had, and his tools, and went on the roof to fix the leak. Unfortu- nately my little half-brother was playing under the ladder and pushed it. It fell and was instantly broken to pieces, locking my uncle on the roof. The worst part was that it started raining again and I had to take a horse and go eight miles before I could get a ladder to help him down.’ (8) (x4 years of age). ‘ As I was walking through the streets one day, I saw a man carrying a package and on the package it said, “Dry goods”. As he was about to enter a house he slipped, the package falling to the ground. When he saw the people laughing at him he turned to pick up the package and there was the Dry Goods flowing all over the street. It sure was Canada Dry Goods.’ (h) (x5 years of age). ‘It was a nice summer day in August, a warm sunny day and I was sitting by my window eating ice cream so as to keep cool, as I had just come from playing a tiresome game and I was reposing. Later on that same day about one o’clock it started to rain, but it was the funniest sight I have ever seen. It was raining on one side of the street and the other was just as dry as a piece of wood in the sun. I was greatly surprised to see this, but later I was told by a man of experience the reason for it.’ (¢) (x5 years of age). ‘ There was a girl whose name was Alice. Her mother had died when she was only three years old and she was living with her aunt, who took care of her from the time her mother died to the present day. One day while her aunt was going to the library it was raining slightly and she caught a cold which made her sick. So the doctor ordered her not to eat anything hard but just to eat eggs, drink milk, etc. It was a Saturday and her aunt said to RAR eae, DREN THE LAUGHTER OF COLOURED CHILDREN 177 / her, ““ Alice, please boil me a soft egg as the doctor said.” Alice got to work and began boiling an egg, but it took so long before her aunt got it, that her aunt said to her, ‘“ Alice, isn't the egg ready yet?” and Alice replied, “ No, aunt, I have been boiling the egg for the last ten minutes and it is still hard.” ’ It is interesting to observe that these fairly typical descriptions of comical sights, in response to the request, ‘Give an account of the funniest sight you have ever seen,” deal mainly with the misfortunes of others or with stupidity as the basis of the humorous situation. Such stories generally make their special appeal to white children at a much earlier stage of their development. The period of rapid growth has far less influence upon the sense of humour of coloured than upon that of white children. The main reason being that the childish outlook is practically maintained right up to the beginning of this stage. Any effect associated with it is, therefore, simply added on to the natural joyousness and exuberance which are typical of very young children. There is no such stable condition, established as the result of authority, in the case of the coloured child, the temporary breakdown of which in the case of the white child produces, as we have frequently seen, such remarkable changes in the mental attitude. Here and there, however, in the essays of coloured children there does appear to be an increased extravagance of language from 12 to 14 years of age, but it is only occasional and not the general rule as in the case of white children. Two very good examples of this are the following from the descriptions of funny sights. In the first, it is an account of a noisy street hawker: ‘ He was making the pedestrians aware of his wares by his loud-pitched voice that held in it an Italian accent.” The second is part of an account of a boy’s early attempt at sleigh riding: ‘ The moment the sleigh reached a piece of rough ground, 178 THE SPRINGS OF LAUGHTER the sleigh stuck in it, and the law of inertia had a tendency to make the body of the youth continue in motion. The surprised lad at once went down the hill until stopped by some external force.’ RETURN TO: EDUCATION/PSYCHOLOGY LIBRARY 2600 Tolman Halt =x 642-4200 LOAN PERIOD 12 3 4 5 6 ALL BOOKS MAY BE RECALLED AFTER 7 DAYS. Return to desk from which they were borrowed. DUE AS STAMPED BELOW. fiy02-ac Ww 4 NV APR 1 FORM NO. DD 14 UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, BERKELEY 5M 9-08 Berkeley, California 94720-6000 [ mt C030kL22521 - » . $ v r RA % FoR YS RECA TST TET PRR ie $ : v » ” » rR Ra —— Cn Re wo. i " 3 : REA PART LPR PET . TE TRNAS sass h A 3 i re 4 FERRY 5 i . 9 Torre SN EARARG Ah h EIA ; 3 a - BRAG Ay p ely #4 ; i ty 4 : chy 2 3 is HoH Si ix ie i SE ar fre a Wh i v i HRP) ESCM Es PVR SRR LOS pe : (RR Ca of SH Col ea TE i SON dak cp wm Th (reer De 4 ari LFA WT) i) Sr Er 3 Eh er La kb aL bok < 4 Be a EN i rE ay Bare ar } N 3 a . 4 Eo) gs Ll s i Car eR Aoki A f i ) : Mn Fos! ’ A ; Fo i ! Ed fi rn # rw Jel i v : fas ) B A Poul 2 Arata wl Sek mui ad Eel bi Fhaaenp Ca Te = 3 " PW! : po ’ at por pr ? Ho > ee EEO 1) isda or ¥ Apri 5 y } $0 fm Abed = 4 fA aH £ hd avheh Ty : he fay on ArH es 3 J 3 % E G $3 he we DIRT TEN * FRA aT yA re pe 3 rr g oF PE ¥ Liar a it Re i fh arhmis ] ki ’ ; i “ 3 ; 7 bh ; : A ye 3 EA Ti Av sod 59% bh i ie sa. Pet y Cn Te podopat wr A ea Faery Fr gh rev TRA eet rs age he BE ao ad vn rr ror ni J Ry PAPE OT rr APHIS 05 Fit oe ha 7] hay Ary 4 or REE A pe « = 3 ! ] ’ x on aie hry Af ot £3 > gee ay re (ere Ey ’ ; 3 E pT frei bl FRE) Lr TR Et reeds oe oh ls ad ot Per . R Re a i Ppp Bet 5 Nod int gE Fs Ff Hane ; Fy LIE Eb Lr Porn Te FT eR & Rr Pr a A oh % i 3 FER INL Sr oh re IW nc Pe . & : visa Pl RAT EA A A Te al ae i A itl [rere Gr dah ARRAS % hot £1°8 DR sh > 4 i Rr IS GRAD aot he : id x i fda Wipf ATPL Ptr 54 Or Grn r pir en $ f rs 3 5 av : . SR Aah mrs na 3 (eed . PR a eran) Ot FR yh AA rr db aii. po {erin ge rs eR A AEP rd RRO, 4 4 Lag BE mem ie 3 Pagid : Cr re Fp yi be 2 ~ o] Ad Fd Ws 0 + Prey gre nny eh 3 Pa 8 By ht J RE j . RENAE Ce po 1 fa . : re " pI 4 f 4 of eiofait op NEL dF RE LW 8 § i . # TARA oid AA ARE hil / 4 Te ! higr, Brn Py hg) sifags gd olp Asd-apellan Fn Ee NOs i CR Sy wy i hl lk ch che dan od Tn hla inal somes Gh me a TO